Hyde 2005 La hipótesis de las Similaridades de Géneros

Septiembre 2005  ● American Psychologist Vol. 60, No. 6, 581–592

La Hipótesis de las Similaridades de Género

 

Janet Shibley Hyde

University of Wisconsin—Madison

 

El modelo de la diferencia, que arguye a favor de que los hombres y las mujeres son vastamente diferentes en lo sicológico, domina los medios populares. Aquí, la autora presenta una idea muy diferente, la hipótesis de las similaridades de género, que sostiene que los varones y las mujeres son similares en la mayoría de  las variables sicológicas, pero no en todas. Los resultados de una reseña de 46 metanálisis dan apoyo a la hipótesis de las similaridades de género. Las diferencias de género varían sustancialmente en magnitud a edades diferentes y dependen del contexto en el que se produce la medición. Las aseveraciones sobreinfladas de diferencias genéricas tienen costos sustanciales en áreas como el lugar de trabajo y las relaciones.

 

Palabras clave: diferencias de género, similaridades de género, metanálisis, agresión

 

Correspondence to Janet Shibley Hyde, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin— Madison, 1202 West Johnson Street, Madison, WI 53706. E-mail: jshyde@wisc.edu

 

Preparation of this article was supported in part by National Science Foundation Grant REC 0207109. I thank Nicole Else-Quest, Sara Lindberg, Shelly Grabe, and Jenni Petersen for reviewing and commenting on a draft of this article.

 

Los medios masivos de comunicación y el público general están cautivados por los descubrimientos de las diferencias de género.Los Hombres Son de Marte, Las Mujeres son de Venus (1992), de John Gray, que argumentaba que existían enormes diferencias sicológicas entre hombres y mujeres, vendió más de 30 millones de ejemplares y fue traducido a 40 idiomas (Gray, 2005). Simplemente No Entiendes: Hombres y Mujeres Conversando (1991), de  Deborah Tannen, argumentaba en favor de la hipótesis de culturas diversas: que los patrones de habla de los hombres y de las mujeres son fundamentalmente tan diferentes que los hombres y las mujeres esencialmente pertenecen a diferentes comunidades o culturas lingüísticas.  Ese libro estuvo en la lista de bestsellers del New York Times por casi cuatro años y ha sido traducido a 24 idomas (AnnOnline, 2005). Estas dos obras, y docenas como ellas, han argumentado a favor de la hipótesis de la diferencia: que hombres y mujeres son, sicológicamente, vastamente diferentes. Aquí presento una vision muy distinta: la hipotésis de similaridades de género (para afirmaciones relacionadas, véanse Epstein, 1988; Hyde, 1985; Hyde & Plant, 1995; Kimball, 1995).

 

La Hipótesis

 

La hipótesis de las similaridades de género sostiene que los hombres y las mujeres son similares en la mayoría de las variables sicológicas, pero no en todas. Esto es, hombres y mujeres, así comoo niños y niñas, son más parecidos que diferentes. En terminus de tamaño de efecto, la hipótesis de similaridades de género afirma que la mayoría de  las diferencias sicológicas de género están en el rango de cerca-de-cero (d ≤ 0.10) o un rango pequeño (0.11 < d < 0.35), unas pocas diferencias sicológicas están en el rango moderado (0.36 < d 1.00).

Although the fascination with psychological gender differences has been present from the dawn of formalized psychology around 1879 (Shields, 1975), a few early researchers highlighted gender similarities. Thorndike (1914), for example, believed that psychological gender differences were too small, compared with within-gender variation, to be important. Leta Stetter Hollingworth (1918) reviewed available research on gender differences in mental traits and found little evidence of gender differences. Another important reviewer of gender research in the early 1900s, Helen Thompson Woolley (1914), lamented the gap between the data and scientists’ views on the question:

The general discussions of the psychology of sex, whether by psychologists or by sociologists show such a wide diversity of points of view that one feels that the truest thing to be said at present is that scientific evidence plays very little part in producing convictions. (p. 372)

 

The Role of Meta-Analysis in Assessing Psychological Gender Differences

 

Reviews of research on psychological gender differences began with Woolley’s (1914) and Hollingworth’s (1918) and extended through Maccoby and Jacklin’s (1974) watershed book The Psychology of Sex Differences, in which they reviewed more than 2,000 studies of gender differences in a wide variety of domains, including abilities, personality, social behavior, and memory. Maccoby and Jacklin dismissed as unfounded many popular beliefs in psychological gender differences, including beliefs that girls are more “social” than boys; that girls are more suggestible; that girls have lower self-esteem; that girls are better at rote learning and simple tasks, whereas boys are better at higher level cognitive processing; and that girls lack achievement motivation. Maccoby and Jacklin concluded that gender differences were well established in only four areas: verbal ability, visual-spatial ability, mathematical ability, and aggression. Overall, then, they found much evidence for gender similarities. Secondary reports of their findings in textbooks and other sources, however, focused almost exclusively on their conclusions about gender differences (e.g., Gleitman, 1981; Lefranc¸ois, 1990).

 

 

Janet ShibleyHyde

 

Shortly after this important work appeared, the statistical method of meta-analysis was developed (e.g., Glass, McGaw, & Smith, 1981; Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Rosenthal, 1991). This method revolutionized the study of psychological gender differences. Meta-analyses quickly appeared on issues such as gender differences in influenceability (Eagly & Carli, 1981), abilities (Hyde, 1981; Hyde & Linn, 1988; Linn & Petersen, 1985), and aggression (Eagly & Steffen, 1986; Hyde, 1984, 1986).

Meta-analysis is a statistical method for aggregating research findings across many studies of the same question (Hedges & Becker, 1986). It is ideal for synthesizing research on gender differences, an area in which often dozens or even hundreds of studies of a particular question have been conducted.

Crucial to meta-analysis is the concept of effect size, which measures the magnitude of an effect—in this case, the magnitude of gender difference. In gender meta-analyses, the measure of effect size typically is d (Cohen, 1988):

 

where MM is the mean score for males, MF is the mean score for females, and sw is the average within-sex standard deviation. That is, d measures how far apart the male and female means are in standardized units. In gender metaanalysis, the effect sizes computed from all individual studies are averaged to obtain an overall effect size reflecting the magnitude of gender differences across all studies. In the present article, I follow the convention that negative values of d mean that females scored higher on a dimension, and positive values of d indicate that males scored higher.

Gender meta-analyses generally proceed in four steps:

(a) The researcher locates all studies on the topic being reviewed, typically using databases such as PsycINFO and carefully chosen search terms. (b) Statistics are extracted from each report, and an effect size is computed for each study. (c) A weighted average of the effect sizes is computed (weighting by sample size) to obtain an overall assessment of the direction and magnitude of the gender difference when all studies are combined. (d) Homogeneity analyses are conducted to determine whether the group of effect sizes is relatively homogeneous. If it is not, then the studies can be partitioned into theoretically meaningful groups to determine whether the effect size is larger for some types of studies and smaller for other types. The researcher could ask, for example, whether gender differences are larger for measures of physical aggression compared with measures of verbal aggression.

 

The Evidence

 

To evaluate the gender similarities hypothesis, I collected the major meta-analyses that have been conducted on psychological gender differences. They are listed in Table 1, grouped roughly into six categories: those that assessed cognitive variables, such as abilities; those that assessed verbal or nonverbal communication; those that assessed social or personality variables, such as aggression or leadership; those that assessed measures of psychological wellbeing, such as self-esteem; those that assessed motor behaviors, such as throwing distance; and those that assessed miscellaneous constructs, such as moral reasoning. I began with meta-analyses reviewed previously by Hyde and Plant (1995), Hyde and Frost (1993), and Ashmore (1990). I updated these lists with more recent meta-analyses and, where possible, replaced older meta-analyses with more up-to-date meta-analyses that used larger samples and better statistical methods.

Hedges and Nowell (1995; see also Feingold, 1988) have argued that the canonical method of meta-analysis— which often aggregates data from many small convenience samples—should be augmented or replaced by data from large probability samples, at least when that is possible (e.g., in areas such as ability testing). Test-norming data as well as data from major national surveys such as the National Longitudinal Study of Youth provide important information. Findings from samples such as these are included in the summary shown in Table 1, where the number of reports is marked with an asterisk.

Inspection of the effect sizes shown in the rightmost column of Table 1 reveals strong evidence for the gender similarities hypothesis. These effect sizes are summarized in Table 2. Of the 128 effect sizes shown in Table 1, 4 were unclassifiable because the meta-analysis provided such a wide range for the estimate. The remaining 124 effect sizes were classified into the categories noted earlier: close-tozero (d ≥ 0.10), small (0.11 < d < 0.35), moderate (0.36 < d < 0.65), large (d = 0.66–1.00), or very large (< 1.00). The striking result is that 30% of the effect sizes are in the close-to-zero range, and an additional 48% are in the small range. That is, 78% of gender differences are

Table 1
Major Meta-Analyses of Research on Psychological Gender Differences
Study and variable Age No. of reports d
Cognitive variables      
Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon (1990)      
Mathematics computation All 45 -0.14
Mathematics concepts All 41 -0.03
Mathematics problem solving All 48 +0.08
Hedges & Nowell (1995)      
Reading comprensión Adolescents 5* -0.09
Vocabulary Adolescents 4* +0.06
Mathematics A Adolescents dolescents 6* +.16
Perceptual speed   4* -0.28
Science Adolescents 4* +0.32
Spatial ability Adolescents 2* +0.19
Hyde, Fennema, Ryan, et al. (1990)      
Mathematics self-confidence All 56 +0.16
Mathematics anxiety All 53 -0.15
Feingold (1988)      
DAT spelling Adolescents 5* -0.45
DAT language Adolescents 5* -0.40
DAT verbal reasoning Adolescents 5* -0.02
DAT abstract reasoning Adolescents 5* -0.04
DAT numerical ability Adolescents 5* -0.10
DAT perceptual speed Adolescents 5* -0.34
DAT mechanical reasoning Adolescents 5* +0.76
DAT space relations Adolescents 5* +0.15
Hyde & Linn (1988)      
Vocabulary All 40 -0.02
Reading comprensión All 18 -0.03
Speech production All 12 -0.33
Linn & Petersen (1985)      
Spatial perception All 62 +0.44
Mental rotation All 29 +0.73
Spatial visualization All 81 +0.13
Voyer et al. (1995)      
Spatial perception All 92 +0.44
Mental rotation All 78 +0.56
Spatial visualization All 116 +0.19
Lynn & Irwing (2004)      
Progressive matrices 6–14 years   15 +0.02
Progressive matrices 15–19 years   23 +0.16
Progressive matrices Adults 10 +0.30
Whitley et al. (1986)      
Attribution of success to ability All 29 +0.13
Attribution of success to effort All 29 -0.04
Attribution of success to task All 29 -0.01
Attribution of success to luck All 29 -0.07
Attribution of failure to ability All 29 +0.16
Attribution of failure to effort All 29 +0.15
Attribution of failure to task All 29 -0.08
Attribution of failure luck All 29 -0.15
Communication      
Anderson & Leaper (1998)      
Interruptions in conversation Adults 53 +0.15
Intrusive interruptions Adults 17 +0.33
Leaper & Smith (2004)      
Talkativeness Children 73 -0.11
Affiliative speech Children 46 -0.26
Assertive speech Children 75 +0.11
Dindia & Allen (1992)      
Self-disclosure (all studies) 205 -0.18
Self-disclosure to stranger 99 -0.07
Self-disclosure to friend 50 -0.28
LaFrance et al. (2003) Smiling

 

Adolescents and adults 418 �0.40    
Smiling: Aware of being observed Adolescents and adults 295 �0.46    
Smiling: Not aware of being observed Adolescents and adults 31 �0.19    
McClure (2000)

 

     
Facial expression processing Infants 29 �0.18 to �0.92    
Facial expression processing Children and adolescents 89 �0.13 to �0.18    
       
       
       
       
       
       
       

 

 

Social and personality variables

Hyde (1984, 1986) Aggression (all types) All 69 �0.50 Physical aggression All 26 �0.60 Verbal aggression All 6 �0.43

Eagly & Steffen (1986) Aggression Adults 50 �0.29 Physical aggression Adults 30 �0.40 Psychological aggression Adults 20 �0.18

Knight et al. (2002) Physical aggression All 41 �0.59 Verbal aggression All 22 �0.28 Aggression in low emotional arousal context All 40 �0.30 Aggression in emotional arousal context All 83 �0.56

Bettencourt & Miller (1996) Aggression under provocation Adults 57 �0.17 Aggression under neutral conditions Adults 50 �0.33

Archer (2004) Aggression in real-world settings All 75 �0.30 to �0.63 Physical aggression All 111 �0.33 to �0.84 Verbal aggression All 68 �0.09 to �0.55 Indirect aggression All 40 �0.74 to �0.05

Stuhlmacher & Walters (1999) Negotiation outcomes Adults 53 �0.09 Walters et al. (1998) Negotiator competitiveness Adults 79 �0.07

Eagly & Crowley (1986) Helping behavior Adults 99 �0.13 Helping: Surveillance context Adults 16 �0.74 Helping: No surveillance Adults 41 �0.02

Oliver & Hyde (1993) Sexuality: Masturbation All 26 �0.96 Sexuality: Attitudes about casual sex All 10 �0.81 Sexual satisfaction All 15 �0.06 Attitudes about extramarital sex All 17 �0.29

Murnen & Stockton (1997) Arousal to sexual stimuli Adults 62 �0.31

Eagly & Johnson (1990) Leadership: Interpersonal style Adults 153 �0.04 to �0.07 Leadership: Task style Adults 154 0.00 to �0.09 Leadership: Democratic vs. autocratic Adults 28 �0.22 to �0.34

Eagly et al. (1992) Leadership: Evaluation Adults 114 �0.05 Eagly et al. (1995) Leadership effectiveness Adults 76 �0.02

Study and variable Age No. of reports d

Social and personality variables (continued)

Eagly et al. (2003) Leadership: Transformational Adults 44 �0.10 Leadership: Transactional Adults 51 �0.13 to �0.27 Leadership: Laissez-faire Adults 16 �0.16

Feingold (1994) Neuroticism: Anxiety Adolescents and adults 13* �0.32 Neuroticism: Impulsiveness Adolescents and adults 6* �0.01 Extraversion: Gregariousness Adolescents and adults 10* �0.07 Extraversion: Assertiveness Adolescents and adults 10* �0.51 Extraversion: Activity Adolescents and adults 5 �0.08 Openness Adolescents and adults 4* �0.19 Agreeableness: Trust Adolescents and adults 4* �0.35 Agreeableness: Tendermindedness Adolescents and adults 10* �0.91 Conscientiousness Adolescents and adults 4 �0.18

Psychological well-being Kling et al. (1999, Analysis I) Self-esteem All 216 �0.21 Kling et al. (1999, Analysis II) Self-esteem Adolescents 15* �0.04 to �0.16 Major et al. (1999) Self-esteem All 226 �0.14 Feingold & Mazzella (1998) Body esteem All — �0.58 Twenge & Nolen-Hoeksema (2002) Depression symptoms 8–16 years 310 �0.02

Wood et al. (1989)

Life satisfaction Adults 17 �0.03

Happiness Adults 22 �0.07 Pinquart & So¨rensen (2001)

Life satisfaction Elderly 176 �0.08

Self-esteem Elderly 59 �0.08

Happiness Elderly 56 �0.06 Tamres et al. (2002)

Coping: Problem-focused All 22 �0.13

Coping: Rumination All 10 �0.19

Motor behaviors
Thomas & French (1985)
Balance 3–20 years 67 �0.09
Grip strength 3–20 years 37 �0.66
Throw velocity 3–20 years 12 �2.18
Throw distance 3–20 years 47 �1.98
Vertical jump 3–20 years 20 �0.18
Sprinting 3–20 years 66 �0.63
Flexibility 5–10 years 13 �0.29
Eaton & Enns (1986)      
Activity level All 127 �0.49
  Miscellaneous    
Thoma (1986)      
Moral reasoning: Stage Adolescents and adults 56 �0.21
Jaffee & Hyde (2000)      
Moral reasoning: Justice orientation All 95 �0.19
Moral reasoning: Care orientation All 160 �0.28
Silverman (2003)      
Delay of gratification All 38 �0.12
Whitley et al. (1999)      
Cheating behavior All 36 �0.17
Cheating attitudes All 14 �0.35 (table continues)
September 2005 ● American Psychologist     585

 

Study and variable Age No. of reports d

Whitley (1997) Computer use: Current All 18 �0.33 Computer self-efficacy All 29 �0.41

Konrad et al. (2000) Job attribute preference: Earnings Adults 207 �0.12 Job attribute preference: Security Adults 182 �0.02 Job attribute preference: Challenge Adults 63 �0.05 Job attribute preference: Physical work environment Adults 96 �0.13 Job attribute preference: Power Adults 68 �0.04

Note. Positive values of d represent higher scores for men and/or boys; negative values of d represent higher scores for women and/or girls. Asterisks indicate that data were from major, large national samples. Dashes indicate that data were not available (i.e., the study in question did not provide this information clearly). No. � number; DAT � Differential Aptitude Test.

small or close to zero. This result is similar to that of Hyde and Plant (1995), who found that 60% of effect sizes for gender differences were in the small or close-to-zero range.

The small magnitude of these effects is even more striking given that most of the meta-analyses addressed the classic gender differences questions—that is, areas in which gender differences were reputed to be reliable, such as mathematics performance, verbal ability, and aggressive behavior. For example, despite Tannen’s (1991) assertions, gender differences in most aspects of communication are small. Gilligan (1982) has argued that males and females speak in a different moral “voice,” yet meta-analyses show that gender differences in moral reasoning and moral orientation are small (Jaffee & Hyde, 2000).

 

Las Excepciones

 

Como se advirtió antes, la hipotesis de similaridades de género no asevera que los hombres y las mujeres son similares en absolutamente todos los dominios. Deben reconocerse las excepciones: }s areas en las que las diferencias de género son moderadas o grandes en magnitud.

Las diferencias más grandes de la Tabla 1 están en el dominio del desempeño motor, particularmente para mediciones tales como la velocidad de arrojar (d = 2.18) y la distancia de arrojar = 1.98) (Thomas & French, 1985). Estas diferencias son particularmente grandes después de la pubertad, cuando la brecha de género en masa muscular y tamaño óseo se amplía.

Una segunda area en la que se encuentran grandes diferencias de género se encuentra en algunas (pero no en todas) las mediciones de la sexualidad (Oliver & Hyde, 1993). Las diferencias de géneros son sorprendentemente grandes para incidencias de masturbación y para actitudes relatives al sexo en una relación casual y sin compromiso. En contraste, la diferencia de género en satisfacción sexual informada está cerca de cero.

A través de varios metanálisis, la agresión repetidamente ha mostrado diferencias de género de magnitud moderada (Archer, 2004; Eagly & Steffen, 1986; Hyde, 1984, 1986). La diferencia de género en agresión física as particularmente confiable y as mayor que la diferencia de género en agresión verbal. Se ha dado mucha publicidad a las diferencias de género en agresión relacional, donde las chicas tienen puntajes más altos (e.g., Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). De acuerdo con el metanálisis de Archer (2004), la agresión indirecta o relacional mostraba un tamaño de efecto para diferencias de género de -0.45 cuando se medía por observación directa, pero era solamente de  -0.19 para puntuaciones de pares, -0.02 para autoinformes, y -0.13 para notificaciones del docente. Por lo tanto, la evidencia relativa a la magnitud de la diferencia de género en agresión relacional es ambigua.

 

Table 2

Effect Sizes (n � 124) for Psychological GenderDifferences, Based on Meta-Analyses, Categorized byRange of Magnitude

Effect size range

Effect sizes 0–0.10 0.11–0.35 0.36–0.65 0.66–1.00 �1.00

Number37 59 19 7 2 %oftotal 30 48 15 6 2

 

The Interpretation of Effect Sizes

 

The interpretation of effect sizes is contested. On one side of the argument, the classic source is the statistician Cohen (1969, 1988), who recommended that 0.20 be considered a small effect, 0.50 be considered medium, and 0.80 be considered large. It is important to note that he set these guidelines before the advent of meta-analysis, and they have been the standards used in statistical power analysis for decades.

In support of these guidelines are indicators of overlap between two distributions. For example, Kling, Hyde, Showers, and Buswell (1999) graphed two distributions differing on average by an effect size of 0.21, the effect size they found for gender differences in self-esteem. This graph is shown in Figure 1. Clearly, this small effect size reflects distributions that overlap greatly—that is, that show more similarity than difference. Cohen (1988) developed a U statistic that quantifies the percentage of nonoverlap of distributions. For d = 0.20, U = 15%; that is, 85% of the areas of the distributions overlap. According to another Cohen measure of overlap, for d = 0.20, 54% of individuals in Group A exceed the 50th percentile for Group B.

 

Graphic Representation of a 0.21 Effect Size

 

 

For another way to consider the interpretation of effect sizes, d can also be expressed as an equivalent value of the Pearson correlation, r (Cohen, 1988). For the small effect size of 0.20, r = .10, certainly a small correlation. A d of 0.50 is equivalent to an r of .24, and for d = 0.80, r = .37.

Rosenthal (1991; Rosenthal & Rubin, 1982) has argued the other side of the case—namely, that seemingly small effect sizes can be important and make for impressive applied effects. As an example, he took a two-group experimental design in which one group is treated for cancer and the other group receives a placebo. He used the method of binomial effect size display (BESD) to illustrate the consequences. Using this method, for example, an r of .32 between treatment and outcome, accounting for only 10% of the variance, translates into a survival rate of 34% in the placebo group and 66% in the treated group. Certainly, the effect is impressive.

How does this apply to the study of gender differences? First, in terms of costs of errors in scientific decision making, psychological gender differences are quite a different matter from curing cancer. So, interpretation of the magnitude of effects must be heavily conditioned by the costs of making Type I and Type II errors for the particular question under consideration. I look forward to statisticians developing indicators that take these factors into account.

Second, Rosenthal used the r metric, and when this is translated into d, the effects look much less impressive. For example, a d of 0.20 is equivalent to an r of 0.10, and Rosenthal’s BESD indicates that that effect is equivalent to cancer survival increasing from 45% to 55%—once again, a small effect. A close-to-zero effect size of 0.10 is equivalent to an r of .05, which translates to cancer survival rates increasing only from 47.5% to 52.5% in the treatment group compared with the control group. In short, I believe that Cohen’s guidelines provide a reasonable standard for the interpretation of gender differences effect sizes.

One caveat should be noted, however. The foregoing discussion is implicitly based on the assumption that the variabilities in the male and female distributions are equal. Yet the greater male variability hypothesis was originally proposed more than a century ago, and it survives today (Feingold, 1992; Hedges & Friedman, 1993). In the 1800s, this hypothesis was proposed to explain why there were more male than female geniuses and, at the same time, more males among the mentally retarded. Statistically, the combination of a small average difference favoring males and a larger standard deviation for males, for some trait such as mathematics performance, could lead to a lopsided gender ratio favoring males in the upper tail of the distribution reflecting exceptional talent. The statistic used to investigate this question is the variance ratio (VR), the ratio of the male variance to the female variance. Empirical investigations of the VR have found values of 1.00–1.08 for vocabulary (Hedges & Nowell, 1995), 1.05–1.25 for mathematics performance (Hedges & Nowell), and 0.87–

1.04 for self-esteem (Kling et al., 1999). Therefore, it appears that whether males or females are more variable depends on the domain under consideration. Moreover, most VR estimates are close to 1.00, indicating similar variances for males and females. Nonetheless, this issue of possible gender differences in variability merits continued investigation.

Developmental Trends

Not all meta-analyses have examined developmental trends and, given the preponderance of psychological research on college students, developmental analysis is not always possible. However, meta-analysis can be powerful for identifying age trends in the magnitude of gender differences. Here, I consider a few key examples of meta-analyses that have taken this developmental approach (see Table 3).

At the time of the meta-analysis by Hyde, Fennema, and Lamon (1990), it was believed that gender differences in mathematics performance were small or nonexistent in childhood and that the male advantage appeared beginning around the time of puberty (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). It was also believed that males were better at high-level mathematical problems that required complex processing, whereas females were better at low-level mathematics that required only simple computation. Hyde and colleagues addressed both hypotheses in their meta-analysis. They found a small gender difference favoring girls in computation in elementary school and middle school and no gender difference in computation in the high school years.

Selected Meta-Analyses Showing Developmental Trends in the Magnitude of Gender Differences

Study and variable Age (years) No. of reports d

Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon (1990)

Mathematics: Complex problem solving 5–10 11 0.00
11–14 21 �0.02
15–18 10 �0.29
19–25 15 �0.32

Kling et al. (1999)

Self-esteem 7–10 22 �0.16
11–14 53 �0.23
15–18 44 �0.33
19–22 72 �0.18
23–59 16 �0.10

�60 6 �0.03 Major et al. (1999)

Self-esteem 5–10 24 �0.01
11–13 34 �0.12
14–18 65 �0.16

19 or older 97 �0.13 Twenge & Nolen-Hoeksema (2002) Depressive symptoms 8–12 86 �0.04 13–16 49 �0.16 Thomas & French (1985) Throwing distance 3–8 — �1.50 to �2.00 16–18 — �3.50

Note. Positive values of d represent higher scores for men and/or boys; negative values of d represent higher scores for women and/or girls. Dashes indicate that data were not available (i.e., the study in question did not provide this information clearly). No. � number.

There was no gender difference in complex problem solving in elementary school or middle school, but a small gender difference favoring males emerged in the high school years (d � 0.29). Age differences in the magnitude of the gender effect were significant for both computation and problem solving.

Kling et al. (1999) used a developmental approach in their meta-analysis of studies of gender differences in self-esteem, on the basis of the assertion of prominent authors such as Mary Pipher (1994) that girls’ self-esteem takes a nosedive at the beginning of adolescence. They found that the magnitude of the gender difference did grow larger from childhood to adolescence: In childhood (ages 7–10), d � 0.16; for early adolescence (ages 11–14), d � 0.23; and for the high school years (ages 15–18), d � 0.33. However, the gender difference did not suddenly become large in early adolescence, and even in high school, the difference was still not large. Moreover, the gender difference was smaller in older samples; for example, for ages 23–59, d � 0.10.

Whitley’s (1997) analysis of age trends in computer self-efficacy are revealing. In grammar school samples, d � 0.09, whereas in high school samples, d � 0.66. This dramatic trend leads to questions about what forces are at work transforming girls from feeling as effective with computers as boys do to showing a large difference in self-efficacy by high school.

These examples illustrate the extent to which the magnitude of gender differences can fluctuate with age. Gender differences grow larger or smaller at different times in the life span, and meta-analysis is a powerful tool for detecting these trends. Moreover, the fluctuating magnitude of gender differences at different ages argues against the differences model and notions that gender differences are large and stable.

 

The Importance of Context

 

Gender researchers have emphasized the importance of context in creating, erasing, or even reversing psychological gender differences (Bussey & Bandura, 1999; Deaux & Major, 1987; Eagly & Wood, 1999). Context may exert influence at numerous levels, including the written instructions given for an exam, dyadic interactions between participants or between a participant and an experimenter, or the sociocultural level.

In an important experiment, Lightdale and Prentice (1994) demonstrated the importance of gender roles and social context in creating or erasing the purportedly robust gender difference in aggression. Lightdale and Prentice used the technique of deindividuation to produce a situation that removed the influence of gender roles. Deindividuation refers to a state in which the person has lost his or her individual identity; that is, the person has become anonymous. Under such conditions, people should feel no obligation to conform to social norms such as gender roles. Half of the participants, who were college students, were assigned to an individuated condition by having them sit close to the experimenter, identify themselves by name, wear large name tags, and answer personal questions. Participants in the deindividuation condition sat far from the experimenter, wore no name tags, and were simply told to wait. All participants were also told that the experiment required information from only half of the participants, whose behavior would be monitored, and that the other half would remain anonymous. Participants then played an interactive video game in which they first defended and then attacked by dropping bombs. The number of bombs dropped was the measure of aggressive behavior.

The results indicated that in the individuated condition, men dropped significantly more bombs (M �31.1) than women did (M �26.8). In the deindividuated condition, however, there were no significant gender differences and, in fact, women dropped somewhat more bombs (M � 41.1) than men (M �36.8). In short, the significant gender difference in aggression disappeared when gender norms were removed.

Steele’s (1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995) work on stereotype threat has produced similar evidence in the cognitive domain. Although the original experiments concerned African Americans and the stereotype that they are intellectually inferior, the theory was quickly applied to gender and stereotypes that girls and women are bad at math (Brown & Josephs, 1999; Quinn & Spencer, 2001; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999; Walsh, Hickey, & Duffy, 1999). In one experiment, male and female college students with equivalent math backgrounds were tested (Spencer et al., 1999). In one condition, participants were told that the math test had shown gender difference in the past, and in the other condition, they were told that the test had been shown to be gender fair—that men and women had performed equally on it. In the condition in which participants had been told that the math test was gender fair, there were no gender differences in performance on the test. In the condition in which participants expected gender differences, women underperformed compared with men. This simple manipulation of context was capable of creating or erasing gender differences in math performance.

Meta-analysts have addressed the importance of context for gender differences. In one of the earliest demonstrations of context effects, Eagly and Crowley (1986) meta-analyzed studies of gender differences in helping behavior, basing the analysis in social-role theory. They argued that certain kinds of helping are part of the male role: helping that is heroic or chivalrous. Other kinds of helping are part of the female role: helping that is nurturant and caring, such as caring for children. Heroic helping involves danger to the self, and both heroic and chivalrous helping are facilitated when onlookers are present. Women’s nurturant helping more often occurs in private, with no onlookers. Averaged over all studies, men helped more (d �0.34). However, when studies were separated into those in which onlookers were present and participants were aware of it, d � 0.74. When no onlookers were present, d ��0.02. Moreover, the magnitude of the gender difference was highly correlated with the degree of danger in the helping situation; gender differences were largest favoring males in situations with the most danger. In short, the gender difference in helping behavior can be large, favoring males, or close to zero, depending on the social context in which the behavior is measured. Moreover, the pattern of gender differences is consistent with social-role theory.

Anderson and Leaper (1998) obtained similar context effects in their meta-analysis of gender differences in conversational interruption. At the time of their meta-analysis, it was widely believed that men interrupted women considerably more than the reverse. Averaged over all studies, however, Anderson and Leaper found a d of 0.15, a small effect. The effect size for intrusive interruptions (excluding back-channel interruptions) was larger: 0.33. It is important to note that the magnitude of the gender difference varied greatly depending on the social context in which interruptions were studied. When dyads were observed, d �0.06, but with larger groups of three or more, d �0.26. When participants were strangers, d �0.17, but when they were friends, d ��0.14. Here, again, it is clear that gender differences can be created, erased, or reversed, depending on the context.

In their meta-analysis, LaFrance, Hecht, and Paluck (2003) found a moderate gender difference in smiling (d �

�0.41), with girls and women smiling more. Again, the magnitude of the gender difference was highly dependent on the context. If participants had a clear awareness that they were being observed, the gender difference was larger (d ��0.46) than it was if they were not aware of being observed (d ��0.19). The magnitude of the gender difference also depended on culture and age.

Dindia and Allen (1992) and Bettencourt and Miller (1996) also found marked context effects in their gender meta-analyses. The conclusion is clear: The magnitude and even the direction of gender differences depends on the context. These findings provide strong evidence against the differences model and its notions that psychological gender differences are large and stable.

Costs of Inflated Claims of Gender Differences

The question of the magnitude of psychological gender differences is more than just an academic concern. There are serious costs of overinflated claims of gender differences (for an extended discussion of this point, see Barnett & Rivers, 2004; see also White & Kowalski, 1994). These costs occur in many areas, including work, parenting, and relationships.

Gilligan’s (1982) argument that women speak in a different moral “voice” than men is a well-known example of the differences model. Women, according to Gilligan, speak in a moral voice of caring, whereas men speak in a voice of justice. Despite the fact that meta-analyses disconfirm her arguments for large gender differences (Jaffee & Hyde, 2000; Thoma, 1986; Walker, 1984), Gilligan’s ideas have permeated American culture. One consequence of this overinflated claim of gender differences is that it reifies the stereotype of women as caring and nurturant and men as lacking in nurturance. One cost to men is that they may believe that they cannot be nurturant, even in their role as father. For women, the cost in the workplace can be enormous. Women who violate the stereotype of being nurturant and nice can be penalized in hiring and evaluations. Rudman and Glick (1999), for example, found that female job applicants who displayed agentic qualities received considerably lower hireability ratings than agentic male applicants (d � 0.92) for a managerial job that had been “feminized” to require not only technical skills and the ability to work under pressure but also the ability to be helpful and sensitive to the needs of others. The researchers concluded that women must present themselves as competent and agentic to be hired, but they may then be viewed as interpersonally deficient and uncaring and receive biased work evaluations because of their violation of the female nurturance stereotype.

A second example of the costs of unwarranted validation of the stereotype of women as caring nurturers comes from Eagly, Makhijani, and Klonsky’s (1992) metaanalysis of studies of gender and the evaluation of leaders. Overall, women leaders were evaluated as positively as men leaders (d � 0.05). However, women leaders portrayed as uncaring autocrats were at a more substantial disadvantage than were men leaders portrayed similarly (d � 0.30). Women who violated the caring stereotype paid for it in their evaluations. The persistence of the stereotype of women as nurturers leads to serious costs for women who violate this stereotype in the workplace.

The costs of overinflated claims of gender differences hit children as well. According to stereotypes, boys are better at math than girls are (Hyde, Fennema, Ryan, Frost, & Hopp, 1990). This stereotype is proclaimed in mass media headlines (Barnett & Rivers, 2004). Meta-analyses, however, indicate a pattern of gender similarities for math performance. Hedges and Nowell (1995) found a d of 0.16 for large national samples of adolescents, and Hyde, Fen-nema, and Lamon (1990) found a d of �0.05 for samples of the general population (see also Leahey & Guo, 2000). One cost to children is that mathematically talented girls may be overlooked by parents and teachers because these adults do not expect to find mathematical talent among girls. Parents have lower expectations for their daughters’ math success than for their sons’ (Lummis & Stevenson, 1990), despite the fact that girls earn better grades in math than boys do (Kimball, 1989). Research has shown repeatedly that parents’ expectations for their children’s mathematics success relate strongly to outcomes such as the child’s mathematics self-confidence and performance, with support for a model in which parents’ expectations influence children (e.g., Frome & Eccles, 1998). In short, girls may find their confidence in their ability to succeed in challenging math courses or in a mathematically oriented career undermined by parents’ and teachers’ beliefs that girls are weak in math ability.

In the realm of intimate heterosexual relationships, women and men are told that they are as different as if they came from different planets and that they communicate in dramatically different ways (Gray, 1992; Tannen, 1991). When relationship conflicts occur, good communication is essential to resolving the conflict (Gottman, 1994). If, however, women and men believe what they have been told—that it is almost impossible for them to communicate with each other—they may simply give up on trying to resolve the conflict through better communication. Therapists will need to dispel erroneous beliefs in massive, unbridgeable gender differences.

Inflated claims about psychological gender differences can hurt boys as well. A large gender gap in self-esteem beginning in adolescence has been touted in popular sources (American Association of University Women, 1991; Orenstein, 1994; Pipher, 1994). Girls’ self-esteem is purported to take a nosedive at the beginning of adolescence, with the implication that boys’ self-esteem does not. Yet meta-analytic estimates of the magnitude of the gender difference have all been small or close to zero: d � 0.21 (Kling et al., 1999, Analysis I), d � 0.04–0.16 (Kling et al., 1999, Analysis II), and d � 0.14 (Major, Barr, Zubek, & Babey, 1999). In short, self-esteem is roughly as much a problem for adolescent boys as it is for adolescent girls. The popular media’s focus on girls as the ones with self-esteem problems may carry a huge cost in leading parents, teachers, and other professionals to overlook boys’ self-esteem problems, so that boys do not receive the interventions they need.

As several of these examples indicate, the gender similarities hypothesis carries strong implications for practitioners. The scientific evidence does not support the belief that men and women have inherent difficulties in communicating across gender. Neither does the evidence support the belief that adolescent girls are the only ones with self-esteem problems. Therapists who base their practice in the differences model should reconsider their approach on the basis of the best scientific evidence.

 

Conclusión

 

La hipótesis de las similaridades de género muestra un fuerte contraste con el modelo de las diferencias, que sostiene que los hombres y las mujeres (y los niños y niñas) son vastamente diferentes sicológicamente. La hipótesis de similaridades de género afirma, en lugar de ello, que los hombres y las mujeres son similares en la mayoría de las variables sicológicas, pero no en todas. Una extensa evidencia de metanálisis de investigación de diferencias de género apoyo la hipótesis de las similaridades de género. Unas pocas excepciones notables son algunas conductas motoras (e.g., la distancia a la que se arroja) y algunos aspectos de la sexualidad, que muestra grandes diferencias de género. La agresión muestra una diferencia de género que es de magnitud moderada.

Es tiempo de considerar los costos de los asertos sobreinflados de las diferencias de género. Se puede argumentar que causan daño en numerosos dominios, incluyendo las oportunidades de la mujer en el lugar de trabajo, conflicto de pareja y comunicación, y análisis de problemas de autoestima entre adolescentes. Lo más importante as que estos asertos no son consistentes con los datos científicos.

 

REFERENCIAS

 

American Association of University Women. (1991). Shortchanging girls, shortchanging America: Full data report. Washington, DC: Author.

Anderson, K. J., & Leaper, C. (1998). Meta-analyses of gender effects on conversational interruption: Who, what, when, where, and how. Sex Roles, 39, 225–252.

AnnOnline. (2005). Biography: Deborah Tannen. Retrieved January 10, 2005, from http://www.annonline.com

Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggression in real-world setting: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology, 8, 291–322.

Ashmore, R. D. (1990). Sex, gender, and the individual. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 486–526). New York: Guilford Press.

Barnett, R., & Rivers, C. (2004). Same difference: How gender myths are hurting our relationships, our children, and our jobs. New York: Basic Books.

Bettencourt, B. A., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 422–447.

Brown, R. P., & Josephs, R. A. (1999). A burden of proof: Stereotype relevance and gender differences in math performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 246–257.

Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713.

Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social–psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710–722.

Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.

Dindia, K., & Allen, M. (1992). Sex differences in self-disclosure: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 106–124.

Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (1981). Sex of researchers and sex-typed communications as determinants of sex differences in influenceability: A meta-analysis of social influence studies. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 1–20.

Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 283–308.

Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & van Engen, M. L. (2003). Transformational, transactional, and laissez-faire leadership styles: A meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 569–591.

Eagly, A. H., & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 233–256.

Eagly, A. H., Karau, S. J., & Makhijani, M. G. (1995). Gender and the effectiveness of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 125–145.

Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., & Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the evaluation of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 3–22.

Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 309–330.

Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.

Eaton, W. O., & Enns, L. R. (1986). Sex differences in human motor activity level. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 19–28.

Epstein, C. F. (1988). Deceptive distinctions: Sex, gender, and the social order. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 43, 95–103.

Feingold, A. (1992). Sex differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A new look at an old controversy. Review of Educational Research, 62, 61–84.

Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 429–456.

Feingold, A., & Mazzella, R. (1998). Gender differences in body image are increasing. Psychological Science, 9, 190–195.

Frome, P. M., & Eccles, J. S. (1998). Parents’ influence on children’s achievement-related perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 435–452.

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Glass, G. V., McGaw, B., & Smith, M. L. (1981). Meta-analysis in social research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Gleitman, H. (1981). Psychology. New York: Norton.

Gottman, J. (1994). Why marriages succeed or fail. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Gray, J. (1992). Men are from Mars, women are from Venus: A practical guide for improving communication and getting what you want in your relationships. New York: HarperCollins.

Gray, J. (2005). John Gray, Ph.D. is the best-selling relationship author of all time. Retrieved January 10, 2005, from http://www.marsvenus.com

Hedges, L. V., & Becker, B. J. (1986). Statistical methods in the metaanalysis of research on gender differences. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 14–50). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Hedges, L. V., & Friedman, L. (1993). Sex differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A reanalysis of Feingold’s results. Review of Educational Research, 63, 95–105.

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995, July 7). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269, 41–45.

Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Hollingworth, L. S. (1918). Comparison of the sexes in mental traits. Psychological Bulletin, 15, 427–432.

Hyde, J. S. (1981). How large are cognitive gender differences? A meta-analysis using �2and d. American Psychologist, 36, 892–901.

Hyde, J. S. (1984). How large are gender differences in aggression? A developmental meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 20, 722–736.

Hyde, J. S. (1985). Half the human experience: The psychology of women (3rd ed.). Lexington, MA: Heath.

Hyde, J. S. (1986). Gender differences in aggression. In J. S. Hyde &

M. C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through metaanalysis (pp. 51–66). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–155.

Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., Ryan, M., Frost, L. A., & Hopp, C. (1990). Gender comparisons of mathematics attitudes and affect: A metaanalysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 14, 299–324.

Hyde, J. S., & Frost, L. A. (1993). Meta-analysis in the psychology of women. In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi (Eds.), Psychology of women: A handbook of issues and theories (pp. 67–103). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.

Hyde, J. S., & Plant, E. A. (1995). Magnitude of psychological gender differences: Another side to the story. American Psychologist, 50, 159–161.

Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703–726.

Kimball, M. M. (1989). A new perspective on women’s math achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 198–214.

Kimball, M. M. (1995). Feminist visions of gender similarities and differences. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.

Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., & Buswell, B. N. (1999). Gender differences in self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 470–500.

Knight, G. P., Guthrie, I. K., Page, M. C., & Fabes, R. A. (2002). Emotional arousal and gender differences in aggression: A meta-analysis. Aggressive Behavior, 28, 366–393.

Konrad, A. M., Ritchie, J. E., Lieb, P., & Corrigall, E. (2000). Sex differences and similarities in job attribute preferences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 593–641.

LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Paluck, E. L. (2003). The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 305–334.

Leahey, E., & Guo, G. (2000). Gender differences in mathematical trajectories. Social Forces, 80, 713–732.

Leaper, C., & Smith, T. E. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children’s language use: Talkativeness, affiliative speech, and assertive speech. Developmental Psychology, 40, 993–1027.

Lefranc¸ois, G. R. (1990). The lifespan (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Lightdale, J. R., & Prentice, D. A. (1994). Rethinking sex differences in aggression: Aggressive behavior in the absence of social roles. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 34–44.

Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.

Lummis, M., & Stevenson, H. W. (1990). Gender differences in beliefs and achievement: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 26, 254–263.

Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2004). Sex differences on the progressive matrices: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 32, 481–498.

Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Major, B., Barr, L., Zubek, J., & Babey, S. H. (1999). Gender and self-esteem: A meta-analysis. In W. B. Swann, J. H. Langlois, & L. A. Gilbert (Eds.) Sexism and stereotypes in modern society: The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence (pp. 223–253). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 424–453.

Murnen, S. K., & Stockton, M. (1997). Gender and self-reported sexual arousal in response to sexual stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Sex Roles, 37, 135–154.

Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51.

Orenstein, P. (1994). Schoolgirls: Young women, self-esteem, and the confidence gap. New York: Anchor Books.

Pinquart, M., & So¨rensen (2001). Gender differences in self-concept and psychological well-being in old age: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 56B, P195–P213.

Pipher, M. (1994). Reviving Ophelia: Saving the selves of adolescent girls. New York: Ballantine Books.

Quinn, D. M., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). The interference of stereotype threat with women’s generation of mathematical problem-solving strategies. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 55–72.

Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (Rev. ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general purpose display of magnitude of experimental effect. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 166–169.

Rudman, L. A., & Glick, P. (1999). Feminized management and backlash toward agentic women: The hidden costs to women of a kinder, gentler image of middle managers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1004–1010.

Shields, S. A. (1975). Functionalism, Darwinism, and the psychology of women: A study in social myth. American Psychologist, 30, 739–754.

Silverman, I. W. (2003). Gender differences in delay of gratification: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 49, 451–463.

Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.

Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.

Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.

Stuhlmacher, A. C., & Walters, A. E. (1999). Gender differences in negotiation outcome: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 52, 653–677.

Tamres, L. K., Janicki, D., & Helgeson, V. S. (2002). Sex differences in coping behavior: A meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 2–30.

Tannen, D. (1991). You just don’t understand: Women and men in conversation. New York: Ballantine Books.

Thoma, S. J. (1986). Estimating gender differences in the comprehension and preference of moral issues. Developmental Review, 6, 165–180.

Thomas, J. R., & French, K. E. (1985). Gender differences across age in motor performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 260–282.

Thorndike, E. L. (1914). Educational psychology (Vol. 3). New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

Twenge, J. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2002). Age, gender, race, socioeconomic status, and birth cohort differences on the Children’s Depression Inventory: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 578–588.

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.

Walker, L. J. (1984). Sex differences in the development of moral reasoning: A critical review. Child Development, 55, 677–691.

Walsh, M., Hickey, C., & Duffy, J. (1999). Influence of item content and stereotype situation on gender differences in mathematical problem solving. Sex Roles, 41, 219–240.

Walters, A. E., Stuhlmacher, A. F., & Meyer, L. L. (1998). Gender and negotiator competitiveness: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 1–29.

White, J. W., & Kowalski, R. M. (1994). Deconstructing the myth of the nonaggressive woman: A feminist analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 487–508.

Whitley, B. E. (1997). Gender differences in computer-related attitudes and behavior: A meta-analysis. Computers in Human Behavior, 13, 1–22.

Whitley, B. E., McHugh, M. C., & Frieze, I. H. (1986). Assessing the theoretical models for sex differences in causal attributions of success and failure. In J. S. Hyde & M. C. Linn (Eds.), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 102–135). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Whitley, B. E., Nelson, A. B., & Jones, C. J. (1999). Gender differences in cheating attitudes and classroom cheating behavior: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 41, 657–677.

Wood, W., Rhodes, N., & Whelan, M. (1989). Sex differences in positive well-being: A consideration of emotional style and marital status. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 249–264.

Woolley, H. T. (1914). The psychology of sex. Psychological Bulletin, 11, 353–379.