Bailey et al 1995 Conducta infantil sexotípica y OS

Developmental Psychology, Volume 31(1), January 1995, p 43–55

[Special Issue: Sexual Orientation and Human Development]

 

Conducta Infantil Sexotípica y orientación sexual: Un Análisis Conceptual y Reseña Cuantitativa

Bailey, J Michael1,3; Zucker, Kenneth J.2

1 Department of Psychology, Northwestern University

2 Child and Adolescent Gender Identity Clinic, Child and Family Studies Centre, Clarke Institute of Psychiatry, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to J. Michael Bailey, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2029, Sheridan Road, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710.

3 una versión temprana de este artículo se presentó en el encuentro de la International Academy of Sex Research, Pacific Grove, California, en junio de 1993.

Agradecemos a Jeff Berman y Robert Nicholson por su consejo en estadística y a Sheri Berenbaum y Ray Blanchard por sus comentarios.

Recibido: 8 de abril de 1993; Revisado: 6 de diciembre de 1993; Aceptado: 9 de diciembre de 1993

 

Resumen

Este artículo reseñó la investigación que examina la asociación entre conducta infantile sexotípica y orientación sexual. Los estudios prospectivos sugieren que el comportamiento sexotípico cruzado [cross-sex-typed behavior] es fuertemente predictivo de orientación sexual adulta en los hombres; no se han ejecutado estudios análogos para mujeres. Aunque son metodológicamente más problemáticos, lose studios retrospectives son útiles para determinar cuántos individuos homosexuales desplegaron comportamiento sexotípico cruzado en la infancia. El cuerpo de estudio retrospectivos, relativamente amplio, que comparan el comportamiento sexotípico de infancia en hombres y mujeres homosexuales y heterosexuales fue reseñado cuantitativamente. Los tamaños de efecto fueron grandes tanto para varones como para mujeres, pero el de los varones fue significativamente más grande. La investigación future debería elaborar las causas de la asociación entre comportamiento infantile sexotípico y orientación sexual, e identificar correlates de diferencias intraorientación en el comportamiento infantile sexotípico.

 

 

La diferenciación sicosexual ha sido un tópico de interés de larga data en la sicología del desarrollo. Tanto la teoría sicoanalítica clásica como la toería del aprendizaje fueron particularmente influyentes en guiar la primera oleada de investigación empírica conducida por desarrollistas (véase, e.g., Mischel, 1966). Los sexólogos también han hecho contribuiones seminales teóricas y empíricas. Por ejemplo, Money (1955) introdujo la expresión rol de género para referirse a “todo aquello que una persona dice o hace para reveler que él o ella tiene el estatus de niño u hombre, niña o mujer, respectivamente. Incluye sexualidad en el sentido de erotismo, pero no se restringe a ella” (p. 254).

A lo largo de las siguientes dos décadas, el rol de género fue descompuesto en tres partes conceptualmente distintas (véase, e.g., Fagot & Leinbach, 1985). Primero,  la identidad de género se distinguió del rol de género. Por ejemplo, Stoller (1964, p. 453) usaba el término levemente diferente identidad de género medular [core gender identity] para describir el  describe a young child’s developing “fundamental sense of belonging to one sex.” Cognitive–developmental psychologists (e.g., Kohlberg, 1966) have used the term gender identity to indicate primarily that a child can accurately discriminate male from female individuals and identify correctly his or her own gender—a task considered by some to be the first stage in gender constancy development.

Compared with Money’s (1955) original definition, the term gender role is now defined more narrowly. Many scholars have used the term to refer to behaviors, attitudes, and personality traits that a society designates as masculine or feminine, that is, more “appropriate” or typical for the male or female social role (cf. Huston, 1983 ; Unger, 1979). In young children, the measurement of gender role behavior includes several easily observable phenomena, including affiliative preference for same versus opposite sex peers, interest in rough-and-tumble play, fantasy roles, toy interests, and dress-up play (see Zucker, 1985). In this article, we use the term sex-typed to refer to those behaviors that have been typically studied as markers of childhood gender identity and gender role.

The third, erotic, component of Money’s (1955) original definition of gender role has also been operationalized more narrowly, most commonly under the rubric of the term sexual orientation. In contemporary sexology, sexual orientation refers to whether a person is more strongly aroused sexually by members of his or her own sex, the opposite sex, or both sexes (homosexual, heterosexual, and bisexual, respectively).

The behavioral markers of gender identity and gender role emerge early, typically by ages 2–4 years, and become “consolidated” thereafter (e.g., Fagot, 1985 ; Huston, 1983). In contrast, sexual orientation appears to be more

readily assessed after puberty, as a person’s sexual interests and desires become more salient (cf. Meyer-Bahlburg, 1980). At least three models have been proposed about the relation between childhood sex-typed behavior and

later sexual orientation. One model hypothesizes a developmental sequence in which gender identity developsbefore gender role, which, in turn, develops before sexual orientation (e.g., Green, 1974, 1987; Meyer-Bahlburg, 1980). In

this model, adult sexual orientation is conceptualized as an end state of psychosexual differentiation, analogous, for example, to formal operations

as an end state of cognitive development. Another model reverses this developmental sequence, positing that sexual orientation is apparent early enough in development to influence the expression of sex-typed behavior (e.g., Isay, 1989). A third model gives less attention to the temporal sequence between these two variables and instead emphasizes the possibility that sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation are both influenced by the same factors, such as prenatal sex hormones (see Green, 1985 ; Zucker, 1990, pp. 12–15).

Although some developmentalists have noted the possible relation between sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation (e.g., Brown, 1957, 1958), others have tended to regard the possibility with skepticism. For example, Serbin (1980)

commented, “there is no evidence that highly sex-typed children are less likely to become homosexual than children showing less extreme sex-role conformity” (p. 85). Storms (1983) remarked, “Gay men are no less likely to have masculine traits than heterosexual men; and lesbians are no less likely to have feminine traits than heterosexual women” (p. 3). And Kohlberg, Ricks, and Snarey (1984)

concluded that “[Existing longitudinal data reveal] little or no correlation between standard measures of [childhood] masculinity–femininity … and … heterosexuality [in adulthood]” (p. 130).

The primary goal of this article was to review the evidence concerning the possible association between childhood sex-typed behavior and adult sexual orientation. The literature that bears on this question contains two types of studies. Prospective studies have begun with children with atypical patterns of sex-typed behavior and followed them into adolescence or adulthood, when their sexual orientation can be assessed by structured interview. Retrospective studies have asked heterosexual and homosexual participants to recall relevant

behaviors and feelings from childhood. There have been far more retrospective than prospective studies. Both types of studies provide unique information, but both also have unique limitations and have been subject to several criticisms. In this article, we summarize and evaluate these criticisms and then provide a meta-analysis of the retrospective studies.

 

Prospective Studies

Prospective designs avoid some of the most important methodological pitfalls of retrospective research (see, e.g., Yarrow, Campbell, & Burton, 1970), particularly the problem of biased recall. Moreover, they can be a crucial test of

the validity of retrospective data (cf. Bowlby, 1969). There are at least two prospective strategies that might yield useful data. One strategy involves the assessment of a large number of unselected children with regard to their sex-typed behavior. As adults, their sexual orientation could be assessed. Although this strategy would be most informative in assessing the association between childhood sex-typed behavior and later sexual orientation, it is also expensive, because a rather large number of participants would be required to obtain a sufficient sample size of adults with a homosexual orientation. Probably for this reason, this strategy has not been used in any prospective study to date.

A second prospective strategy is to ascertain a sample of youngsters believed to be disproportionately likely to develop the outcome of interest (along the lines of studies of children “at risk” for a major mental disorder, such as schizophrenia [e.g., Watt, Anthony, Wynne, & Rolf, 1984]). This design can considerably reduce the number of participants that need to be studied. In studies of psychosexual differentiation, this strategy has involved identifying children (typically boys) who displayed marked patterns of cross-gender behavior. Many of these children would meet the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM–IV (4th. ed.; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) diagnostic criteria for gender identity disorder ( ). These studies examined the relation between childhood cross-gender identification and later psychosexuality, especially transsexualism and homosexuality (see, e.g., Green, 1974).

has conducted the most comprehensive prospective study of boys with marked patterns of childhood cross-gender behavior. This study contained a sample of 66 feminine and 56 control boys assessed initially at a mean age of 7.1 years (range = 4–12 years). About two thirds of the boys in each group were followed long enough so that their sexual orientation could be assessed in late adolescence ( = 18.9 years; range = 14–24 years). Data from a semistructured clinical interview were used to rate sexual orientation in fantasy and behavior on , pp. 636–641) 7-point sexual orientation continuum, where 0 = and 6 = . Depending on the measure (fantasy or behavior), 75%–80% of the previously feminine boys were either bisexual or homosexual (Kinsey ratings of 2–6) at followup as compared with 0%–4% of the control boys.

Green (1974, 1987)

M

Kinsey, Pomeroy, and Martin’s (1948

exclusive heterosexuality exclusive homosexuality

results were similar to those of six other follow-up reports of boys who displayed marked crossgender

behavior ( ; ; ; ; ; ). As summarized by , these reports contained 55 boys seen at follow-up, usually in late adolescence or young adulthood (range = 13–36 years [for details, see ]). At follow-up, 5 boys were classified as transsexual (all of whom had a homosexual sexual orientation), 21 as homosexual, 15 as heterosexual, and 14 could not be rated with regard to sexual orientation. Excluding these last 14, 26 of 41 boys (63%) had homosexual orientations. Overall, then, there is clear evidence of a relation between patterns of childhood sex-typed behavior and later sexual orientation.

Green’s (1987)

Bakwin, 1968 Davenport, 1986 Kosky, 1987 Lebovitz, 1972 Money & Russo, 1979 Zuger, 1984

Zucker (1990)

Zucker, 1985, 1990

Although these prospective data confirm a link between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation for boys, several qualifications need to be made. To date, insufficient numbers of girls have been followed prospectively to draw definitive conclusions about the fit with retrospective studies. Second, the prospective studies consisted largely of clinic-referred children who displayed marked patterns of cross-gender behavior, including gender dysphoria. These children might be conceptualized as falling at the extreme end of a continuum

of cross-gender identification. The extent of cross-gender behavior recollected by most homosexual adults is probably not as extreme as that displayed by children who meet diagnostic criteria for gender identity disorder (Friedman, 1988). For these reasons, retrospective studies are superior to prospective studies in determining whether, in general, homosexual individuals exhibit atypical sex-typed behavior during childhood.

DSM–IV

 

Retrospective Studies

Since the early 1960s, researchers of diverse theoretical persuasions have collected, by various methods and measures, recall data on components of childhood gender identity and role in men and women with either an exclusive or predominant heterosexual or homosexual sexual orientation. With a few exceptions, this work has been conducted in the United States and Canada. Every study that we located showed, at least on some measures, a significant difference between heterosexual and homosexual adults in their recall of childhood gender identity and role patterns, with homosexual adults more likely to recall atypical (i.e., statistically uncommon) patterns of childhood sex-typed behavior. Despite the remarkable consistency in findings across studies, the validity of the evidence linking sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation has been questioned. There have been at least three types of criticisms: concerns about sampling techniques, the lack of “perfect” correlations, and threats to the validity of recall data.

Sampling Methods

Kohlberg et al. (1984) commented on the use of samples of “homosexuals in trouble (prison or psychotherapy)” (p.129). Although the heterosexual controls in such studies were also in prison or in psychotherapy, the use of

nonrepresentative samples has been recognized as a potential problem (Harry, 1986). But over the years, the empirical studies that have assessed recall data on sex-typed behavior have also sampled many subjects who were neither in prison nor in psychotherapy (see Zucker, 1987). No one has tested the hypothesis that sampling bias affects the differences in recalled childhood sex-typed behavior between heterosexual and homosexual adults.

Imperfect Correlations

questioned the evidence establishing a relation between sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation because some adults with a homosexual sexual orientation do not recall any cross-gender behavior during childhood and some adults with a heterosexual sexual orientation do. But this merely means that the association between sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation, if it exists, is not a perfect one. Thus, another aim of the present study was to describe quantitatively the strength of the relation between sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation, which can help in interpreting the importance of group differences (cf. ).

Carrier (1986)

Hyde, 1990

Validity of Recall Data

The most common criticism of the retrospective studies concerns memory distortion or selective recall. Ross (1980)advanced a particularly strong version of the retrospective distortion hypothesis: Homosexual adults did not really have cross-gender traits or behaviors in childhood but merely remembered themselves that way because they have internalized societal stereotypes (Ross, 1980 ; cf. Hoult, 1983/1984; Kite & Deaux, 1987 ; Risman & Schwartz, 1988). There is no direct empirical support for the retrospective distortion hypothesis. study, while often cited as supporting the hypothesis (see, e.g., Hoult, 1983/1984 ; Ross, 1984), Ross’s (1980) did not show that homosexual adults’ recollections were affected by beliefs about homosexuality and gender roles; in fact, it did not even examine sexual orientation differences in childhood cross-gender behavior. The study merely showed that gay men from Sweden were less likely than gay men from Australia (a more conservative culture than Sweden with respect to gender roles) to believe in such an association.

Selective recall of a less complete nature is also possible. Perhaps as a result of social stereotypes, homosexual adults are more likely than heterosexual adults to recall patterns of childhood cross-gender behavior. This could occur if either homosexual adults overreported, or heterosexual adults underreported, atypical sex-typed behavior. For example, homosexual adults familiar with scientific theories about the relation between childhood

sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation might provide exaggerated memories of sex-atypical behavior (

). , pp. 120–121), however, found no differences in extent of recalled “gender

nonconformity” between gay men and women familiar with scientific writings versus those who were not, suggesting that that particular version of retrospective distortion cannot account for the sexual orientation differences. Similarly, heterosexual adults may be more likely to forget sex-atypical behavior. However, studies of relevant behaviors suggest that most children have little sex-atypical behavior to forget. Unselected samples of children typically show same-sex gender role preferences, and very few persistently wish to be of the other sex

(Huston, 1983 ; Sandberg, Meyer-Bahlburg, Ehrhardt, & Yager, 1993; Zucker, 1985). Differential selective recall has not been empirically demonstrated to magnify sexual orientation differences in childhood memories; however, it remains the most serious concern regarding the validity of the retrospective findings.

Harry (1984

Another problem pertains to simple forgetting or ( ). As a result of an

immature memory system ( ), some adults may be unable to retrieve specific memories

pertaining to gender behavior from the preschool years, when such behavior first appears ( ; ).

Thus, specific memories tapped by retrospective studies are most likely from after the preschool years. Given the

stability of children’s gender role preferences ( ), however, adult retrospections are probably based on

an aggregation of multiple displays of relevant behaviors rather than the “one-moment-in-time” events discussed

by , pp. 312–313). In any case, this memory limitation should be unrelated to sexual

orientation.

childhood amnesia Pillemer & White, 1989

Pillemer & White, 1989

Fagot, 1985 Huston, 1983

Huston, 1983

Pillemer and White (1989

Because the recall of childhood events are no doubt imperfect, investigators have attempted to develop

methods of checking the accuracy of such data. One strategy has been to use multiple informants (see,

e.g., ). found statistically significant, albeit modest,

positive correlations between the recall of childhood gender behavior on the part of young adult homosexual and

heterosexual men and women and that of their mothers. Although this finding deserves replication, it provides

some evidence for the validity of the participant’s own recollections.

Brewin, Andrews, & Gotlib, 1993 Bailey, Miller, and Willerman (1993)

In this article, we provide a quantitative review of the retrospective studies. The mean effect size from such a

review provides a rough indicator of how large the differences are. Furthermore, there is a sufficiently large

number of retrospective studies to allow the examination of factors that might affect the magnitude of effect sizes.

 

Method

Study Selection

Retrospective studies selected for analysis were drawn from the English-language literature. Two criteria were used for inclusion: (a) The age period for which relevant behaviors were to be recalled was childhood, typically

operationalized in relevant measures as either before puberty or <=12 years of age, and (b) relevant data were available for both a homosexual and a same-sex heterosexual group.

Search procedures included an issue-by-issue search of all volumes of

, and . Two bibliographies on research pertaining to homosexuality (

; ) were also inspected. In addition, we conducted a computer search of the literature using PsycLIT, Medline, and Dissertation Abstracts International (through 1992) using the keywords

, and . These keywords were then crossed with the keywords

, and . We also examined articles cited in the bibliographies of relevant

studies. Finally, we examined all abstracts of papers presented at the annual meetings of the International

Academy of Sex Research. In two instances ( ; ), we needed to contact authors to obtain data

useful for the meta-analyses. Although another study ( ) collected relevant data, they were no longer

available, and so the study was discarded from our analysis. Altogether, there were 41 citations that met the

inclusion criteria and that included the necessary data for analysis.

Archives of Sexual Behavior, Journal of

Sex Research Journal of Homosexuality Dynes,

1987 Weinberg & Bell, 1972

homosexuality,

gay, lesbian sexual orientation gender identity,

gender role, sex role transsexualism

Caretto, 1991 Guloien, 1983

Tweedy, 1984

Coding

Study effect sizes.

Effect sizes were computed by using ( ), calculated as the difference between the means of the

homosexual and same-sex heterosexual groups divided by their pooled standard deviations. Because is a biased

estimator, we used , Formula 4) unbiasing correction for all effect sizes. Effect sizes were scaled so

that positive values indicate greater recalled cross-gender behavior and identity among the homosexual groups.

d Cohen, 1988

d

Hedges’s (1982

Our decision to use the pooled standard deviation requires some justification because, as we show later, the

homosexual and heterosexual standard deviations differed from each other. have

argued that in such cases, one should choose either standard deviation, rather than the pooled standard deviation,

because effect sizes computed with the former approach are more meaningful. We rejected that approach for two

reasons. First, because (as we also show later) the distributions were not both normal, there can be no

straightforward translation from to a more meaningful description, such as percentage nonoverlap, commonly

provided in meta-analyses. Thus, in the present case, is not very meaningful regardless of the standard

deviation used to compute it. Second, because it is computed by using the entire sample, the pooled standard

deviation provides the most stable estimate of . This is important because one primary goal of this meta-analysis

is to examine whether various factors (e.g., sex) influence the magnitude of . The more reliably is estimated,

the more powerful the test of any such factor will be.

Glass, McGaw, and Smith (1981)

d

d

d

d d

Most effect sizes were calculated from means and standard deviations reported in the studies. When this

information was unavailable, we used other statistics to estimate the effect size (see Glass et al., 1981, chap. 5).

Some studies reported results for scales of sex-typed behavior. Others reported individual item data, and a few

studies reported results for both overall scales and individual items. Because different effect sizes from the same

study are not independent, we computed one overall effect size for each study as follows: If results were given

for overall scales, these were used because scale scores are more reliable (and hence should yield more stable

estimates) than individual items. If only individual item data were given, then effect sizes were computed for all

relevant items and then averaged to yield the overall effect size.1

A total of 48 independent effect sizes were computed for this review’s 41 different citations: 16 for women and

32 for men. Seven citations included relevant data for both men and women. We use the term to refer to

each independent effect size. Thus, citations that include data for men and women count as two studies.

study

 

Study characteristics.

We coded several study characteristics to investigate their influence on the magnitude of effect sizes. One factor that we hypothesized would be important was the type of measure used in the study. Some studies reported results on the basis of multi-item scales, whereas others reported results separately for each item examined. Because scale measures are more reliable, they should yield larger effect sizes than individual item measures. Thus, we distinguished studies that used multi-item scales from those that reported data only at the item level.

Sex was also considered, because we hypothesized that effect sizes would be larger for men than for women. Masculine behavior in young girls appears to be more common than feminine behavior in young boys (Huston, 1983).

Several other study characteristics were examined. Year of publication was included to investigate cohort effects. The average age of the sample participants was used to investigate possible age effects on memory. Ascertainment method refers to whether homosexual participants were volunteers from the community or were recruited as clients of mental health professionals. The first author’s discipline from each study was dichotomized into medical versus nonmedical. We analyzed the importance of the first author’s sexual orientation, dichotomized into heterosexual versus nonheterosexual. This analysis, requested by a reviewer, is useful in examining the importance of unmeasured biases (e.g., ideology) that might be related to authors’ sexual orientation and influence the magnitude of results. Finally, we coded whether each effect size was computed by using published or unpublished data to investigate whether the unpublished work that we found was unrepresentative. This was particularly important because four of the seven effect sizes from unpublished data were contributed by one of us (JMB). Table 1 gives frequencies for some relevant predictor variables.

Table 1 Frequencies of Study Attributes

 

Item content.

Item-level data were available for 28 studies. Effect sizes were coded for the 265 items relevant to childhood sex-typed behavior. Besides using these effect sizes to compute study effect sizes when multi-item scale data

were not available, we also examined item content. This allowed us to characterize the measures used in the studies in more detail. Furthermore, we examined the effect sizes of different types of items. We coded all of the items according to two separate classifications. The first had seven categories: (a) roughand-tumble play, competitive athletics, or aggression; (b) toy and activity preference; (c) imagined roles, careers, or role models; (d) cross-dressing; (e) preference for affiliating with male versus female individuals; (f) social

reputation as a “sissy,” “tomboy,” or “loner”; and (g) gender identity. Two raters independently assigned each

item to exactly one category, with good interrater reliability ([kappa] = .87). Only items that both raters assigned

to the same categories were coded for later analysis.

The second classification scheme grouped items according to whether they concerned masculine or feminine

behavior. For example, the item “I liked wearing dresses” concerns feminine behavior, whereas the item “I liked

football” concerns masculine behavior. Some items were neither strictly masculine nor strictly feminine in

content (e.g., “I preferred masculine to feminine activities”). Two raters independently rated each item as

, or . Interrater reliability was high ([kappa] = .91). Only items that both raters agreed

were either masculine or feminine were coded for later analysis.

masculine, feminine neither

Each study contributed at most one observation to each analysis of items. If a study had more than one item in

any category, the effect sizes for those items were averaged to give the study’s effect size for the category. Thus,

each study yielded at most nine item-level effect sizes: one for each of the seven categories from the first

classification scheme, a masculine score, and a feminine score.

 

Results

Sample sizes ranged from 34 to 8,751, with a median of 189. The proportion of participants in a study who

were homosexual ranged from .025 to .886, with a median of .50. (Both extremes were from large studies, so

that even the less frequent group was adequately represented in them.) The total number of participants studied

included 8, 963 heterosexual women, 1,729 lesbians, 11,298 heterosexual men, and 5, 734 gay men. After the

large samples from the two Kinsey studies ( ; ) were excluded, the

respective figures were 1,583, 1,539, 3,315, and 4,966. Mean age of the heterosexual subsamples was 27.4 years

( = 5.8); for homosexual subsamples, it was 29.8 years ( = 4.7).

Kinsey et al., 1948 Kinsey, Pomeroy, Martin, & Gebhard, 1953

SD SD

Effect sizes ranged from 0.50 to 2.09, with a mean of 1.19 ( = 0.43), which was significantly greater than

zero, (47) = 19.2, < .001. The mean of effect sizes computed using multi-item measures, 1.37 ( = 0.38),

was significantly greater than that for effect sizes computed as the average of single item effects, 0.87 ( =

0.32), (46) = 4.7, < .001. Type of measure accounted for a substantial proportion of variance in effect size (

= .32).

SD

t p SD

SD

t p R2

We investigated the influence of the other factors on the magnitude of effect sizes using a regression approach.

First, for each factor, we estimated a simple regression model with that factor as the sole predictor of effect size.

This allowed us to estimate the importance of each predictor ignoring all other predictors. Second, we estimated a

multiple regression model with both the factor of interest and type of measure (i.e., multi-item scales vs. single

items) as predictors. Because type of measure was substantially associated with effect size, including it as a

covariate should provide a more powerful test of other factors uncorrelated with it. Results from these analyses

are provided in Table 2.

Table 2 Total and Incremental Variance in Effect Size Accounted for by Study Characteristics

Sex was a significant predictor of effect size, both alone, = .14, (1, 46) = 7.7, < .01, and adjusted for

type of measure, [DELTA] = .10, (1, 45) = 7.5, < .01. The mean effect size for men, 1.31 ( = 0.43),

exceeded that for women, 0.96 ( = 0.35). Jointly, sex and type of measure accounted for 42% of the total

variance in effect sizes. No other predictor was significant either by itself or when type of measure was

controlled, though two approached significance. When type of measure was controlled, effect sizes for published

studies were larger than those for unpublished studies, [DELTA] = .05, (1, 45) = 3.6, = .06. This may

partly reflect the relatively high proportion of unpublished studies of women, which showed smaller effects (57%

of unpublished studies vs. 29% of published studies). Again, when type of measure was controlled, effect sizes

R2 F p

R2 F p SD

SD

R2 F p

were larger for studies whose first authors were physicians, [DELTA] = .03, (1, 45) = 2.3, = .14.

However, author’s discipline was also confounded with sex of subjects, with 80% of physicians’ studies about

men compared with 63% of studies by authors from nonmedical disciplines.

R2 F p

Distributions of Sex-Typed Behavior

Effect sizes are sometimes used to generate indexes of nonoverlap between two distributions (e.g.,

), which can be easily obtained by using statistical tables (e.g., , p. 22) provided that both

distributions are normal and have equal variances. Both assumptions are untenable for studies in the present

review. Frequency distributions were available for 12 studies. In every one of these studies, the heterosexual

distribution was positively skewed ( < .001). The coefficient of skewness for heterosexual distributions ranged

from 0.14 to 2.50, with a median of 1.01. Because the heterosexual distributions were positively skewed, they

were not normal. In contrast, skewness coefficients for the homosexual distributions ranged from -.47 to .55,

with a median of -.20, and were in all cases less than the heterosexual distributions from the same studies. Thus,

compared with the homosexual distributions, the heterosexual distributions were weighted relatively more toward

the low end, suggesting a floor effect. This possibility was further supported by an examination of the relative

variability of the two groups. Standard deviations were available for 24 studies. In every study, the ratio of the

homosexual to heterosexual standard deviation ( ) exceeded 1 (median = 1.37), a highly significant excess ( <

.001).

Smith, Glass, &

Miller, 1980 Cohen, 1988

p

n n p

Because nonnormality precluded computation of indexes of nonoverlap using tables, we elected to estimate

nonoverlap directly from available data, as follows: Frequency distributions were available for 5 female and 7

male samples given multi-item scales (analyses were restricted to data that used multi-item scales because they

should be least affected by measurement error). For each pair of same-sex distributions from the same study, the

homosexual and heterosexual subsamples were weighted to equate their sample sizes, and the combined groups’

scores were standardized. Equating the subsample sizes maximized the comparability of standard scores across

studies because otherwise, the means and standard deviations used to compute standard scores would reflect the

ratio of homosexual to heterosexual subjects, which differed between studies. All female samples were weighted

to equate their sample sizes, and their distributions were pooled. The male samples were treated similarly and

then were pooled to form a separate distribution. The resulting distributions thus represent an unweighted

average of the constituent samples.

Z

Figures and represent the frequency distributions of the pooled samples of women and men, respectively.

The effect sizes associated with the two distributions were 1.02 (for women) and 1.53 (for men). These were

similar to the effect sizes predicted by regression for studies that used multi-item scales (1.17 and 1.45,

respectively); thus, they appear to be fairly representative of the studies examined herein. The female distribution

indicated that 81% of lesbians exceeded the heterosexual female median and that 12% of heterosexual women

scored exceeded the homosexual female median. There was slightly less overlap for men, reflecting the larger

effect size for the male distribution, with 89% of gay men exceeding the heterosexual median, and only 2% of

heterosexual men scoring above the homosexual median.

1 2

Figure 1. Frequency distributions of recalled childhood sex-typed behavior for composite distributions of homosexual and heterosexual women. (See

text for details of how composite distributions were assembled.)

Ovid: Bailey: Dev Psychol, Volume 31(1).January 1995.43–55 5/14/03 7:25 AM

https://snap.it.northwestern.edu/p/p.cgi/ovidcom/gateway1:80/ovidweb.cgi Page 11 of 23

Figure 2. Frequency distributions of recalled sex-typed behavior for composite distributions of homosexual and heterosexual men. (See text for details

of how composite distributions were assembled.)

Item Content

contains the mean effect size for each category of items, separately for men and women. All mean effect

sizes were significantly greater than zero (two-tailed tests), with one exception: “Careers/role models” for

women, (3) = 2.3, = .11 ( = 0.67, = 0.59), which was based on only four studies.

Table 3

t p M SD

Table 3 Mean Effect Size by Category of Item

Because the first coding scheme had seven categories, there were too few studies to sustain a statistically

powerful test that the categories differed in their average effect sizes. In contrast, it was possible to compare the

two categories in the second coding scheme. Specifically, we examined, separately for men and women, whether

homosexual and same-sex heterosexual subjects differed more on items assessing masculine behavior or on those

focusing on feminine behavior. To test this hypothesis, we performed paired tests over all studies that had both

a feminine and a masculine effect size. The result was not significant for women, (6) = -0.8, = .47, but was

marginally significant for men, (12) = -1.8, = .10, who showed a tendency for larger effect sizes on more

masculine-oriented items.

t

t p

t p

 

Discussion

As our analyses demonstrated for both men and women, research has firmly established that homosexual subjects recall substantially more cross-sex-typed behavior in childhood than do heterosexual subjects. By rough criteria, effect sizes were large for both men and women. Indeed, they were among the largest effect sizes ever reported in the realm of sex-dimorphic behaviors. summarized the effect sizes from several meta-analytic studies of behaviors often assumed to show significant sex differences: verbal ability ( = -.11),

mathematical ability ( = .43), components of spatial ability ( s ranged from .13 to .73), and aggression ( = .50). The effect sizes obtained in the present study exceeded those figures by a wide margin (see also ).

Cohen’s

(1988)

Hyde (1990)

d

d d d

Eaton & Enns, 1986

Effect sizes were larger for men than for women. Because female homosexuality appears to be less prevalent

than male homosexuality ( ), childhood cross-gender behavior is likely to be less predictive of female

than of male homosexuality. To illustrate, assume estimates for the prevalence of female and male

homosexuality, 1.5% and 4%, respectively. Suppose that a child shows a degree of cross-sex-typed behavior that

Gebhard, 1972

Gebhard’s (1972)

Ovid: Bailey: Dev Psychol, Volume 31(1).January 1995.43–55 5/14/03 7:25 AM

https://snap.it.northwestern.edu/p/p.cgi/ovidcom/gateway1:80/ovidweb.cgi Page 12 of 23

is typical of those who will become homosexual adults (i.e., at the median of the homosexual distribution). With the use of our results concerning nonoverlap to extrapolate to the general population, 0.75% (.50*.015) of girls both have the requisite degree of cross-sex-typed behavior and will become lesbians, compared with 11.82%

(.12*.985) of girls who are equally cross-gendered and will become heterosexual women. Thus, only about 6% (.0075/[.1182 + .0075]) of such girls will become homosexual. Analogous computations suggest that 51% of

boys with the requisite degree of cross-sex-typed behavior will become homosexual. Thus, early cross-gender behavior appears to be substantially more predictive of homosexuality in men than in women. Because the

expected rate of lesbianism is fairly low, prospective studies of masculine girls may require larger samples than those of feminine boys to achieve adequate statistical power.

These calculations also have relevance to the generality of available prospective studies of boys. The expected rate of adult homosexuality for boys at the homosexual median, 51%, is lower than has been obtained in prospective studies of feminine boys (e.g., Green, 1987; Zuger, 1984), suggesting that boys referred to clinics for crosssex-typed behavior are more extreme in that respect than are typical prehomosexual boys. On the other hand, the two rates are not so different as to suggest that the findings for the clinic-referred boys are entirely inapplicable to prehomosexual boys in general.

The other factor significantly associated with magnitude of effect size, type of measurement, is less theoretically interesting. Psychometric theory clearly predicts that unreliable items should yield smaller effect sizes than more reliable scales, and this was strongly confirmed. One lesson from this meta-analysis, therefore, is the desirability of using multi-item scales. Even if a researcher wanted to distinguish different aspects of sextyped behavior (e.g., to see which of them is the best predictor of sexual orientation), the optimal strategy is to use multi-item scales to measure them.

Because a strong empirical link between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation has been established for men in both prospective and retrospective studies, it is likely to be genuine. Although it is logically possible that the prospective findings were due to an extremely unrepresentative group of boys and the retrospective findings to memory bias, a more parsimonious interpretation is that both kinds of findings reflect a rather strong association between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation. Retrospective findings for women have not yet been supported by prospective ones. Although we believe that prospective studies of masculine girls are desirable, we know of no strong empirical reason to be skeptical of the retrospective findings.

On the other hand, the possibility remains that retrospective studies exaggerate the true difference between homosexual and heterosexual participants in childhood sex-typed behavior, because of memory bias, and so the

effect sizes we computed should be regarded cautiously.

Historical and Cross-Cultural Limitations

The retrospective studies we reviewed were limited with respect to both time and place. With the exception of Kinsey’s two studies (Kinsey et al., 1948; Kinsey et al., 1953), all of the studies were published after 1960. On the other hand, the sample cohorts spanned a period during which important changes occurred regarding the status and conception of homosexuality. Some of the homosexual adult samples were from age cohorts born before the emergence of the modern gay subculture. Because year of publication was uncorrelated with effect size, homosexual–heterosexual differences in childhood sex-typed behavior appear to have been stable across the three decades during which the bulk of the studies were conducted.

Almost all of the retrospective studies consisted of homosexual and heterosexual adults from Western industrialized countries, specifically the United States, Canada, and Australia. Only Whitam and Mathy (1986, 1991) studied participants from other countries (Brazil, Guatemala, Peru, and the Phillipines). Although effect sizes for these studies were similar to those containing only Western samples, the generality of the association between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation remains an open question. Whitam’s sampling techniques have been criticized (Risman & Schwartz, 1988), and no one to our knowledge has attempted to replicate his findings.

Herdt (1990) has described age-structured homosexuality among Papua New Guinea men in the Sambia tribe.         Beginning in middle childhood, boys are introduced to homosexual activity (fellatio) with late adolescent and young adult males as a rite of passage to adult heterosexuality. There is no evidence that childhood cross-sextyped behavior is associated with this form of homosexuality. Note, however, that such homosexual behavior among the Sambia is probably unrelated to homosexuality as a sexual orientation, which is the sense that we have emphasized herein. We are unaware of any culture in which sexual orientation is unrelated to childhood sex-typed behavior, although cross-cultural similarities and differences in the development of sexual orientation remain an important, understudied area of inquiry.

Nature of the Link Between Childhood Sex-Typed Behavior and Sexual Orientation

Why is there an association between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation? Two general approaches to this question have been explored, one primarily biological and the other psychosocial.

 

Biological interpretations.

The most prominent biological hypothesis is that sexual orientation is a function of the degree of masculinization and defeminization of relevant neural structures due to prenatal or early postnatal effects of androgens (e.g., Dörner, 1976; Meyer-Bahlburg, 1984). This theory could account for the association between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation in at least two ways. First, the relevant neural structures affecting adult sexual orientation might be the same structures that, in childhood, affect sex-typed behavior. The hypothalamus has been the most frequently mentioned brain area hypothesized to affect sexual orientation. The first biological explanation for the association between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation, then, would also require that the hypothalamus influence other sex-dimorphic behavior. A second possible explanation is that the processes that masculinize or defeminize the neural structures affecting sexual orientation also have more general effects. Thus, for example, the hypothalamus may affect sexual orientation whereas a separate area, subject to similar influences during sexual differentiation, affects childhood sex-typed behavior. The plausibility of this explanation is supported somewhat by findings of LeVay (1991) and Allen and Gorski (1992) , who demonstrated associations with male sexual orientation for two different areas of the brain.

The general hypothesis that childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation are subject to similar hormonal influences (whether or not they are affected by the same brain structures) has received empirical support from studies of girls and women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), which results in prenatal and early postnatal exposure to high levels of androgens. Girls and women with CAH have been found to be somewhat masculine with regard to several components of sex-typed behavior (e.g., ; ). Adult women with CAH also recall masculine patterns of childhood sex-typed behavior ( ; ) and appear to have increased rates of bisexuality and homosexuality, particularly in fantasy ( ; ; ; ; but see ).

Berenbaum & Hines, 1992 Ehrhardt

& Baker, 1974 Dittmann et

al., 1990 Zucker et al., 1992

Dittmann, Kappes, & Kappes, 1992 Ehrhardt, Evers, & Money, 1968 Money, Schwartz, & Lewis, 1984 Zucker et al., 1992

Mulaikal, Migeon, & Rock, 1987

 

Psychosocial interpretations.

Psychosocial interpretations of the association between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation have focused primarily on two interrelated processes: the influence of parent–child relationships on identification with parents and differential reinforcement of sex-typed behavior.

Psychoanalysts have stressed the child’s relative identification with the same-sex or opposite-sex parent as a precursor to eventual sexual orientation (e.g., Bieber et al., 1962). Much of this literature has focused on men, in whom homosexuality was hypothesized to result from the combination of an excessively close mother–son relationship and a distant, if not antagonistic, father–son relationship. This pattern allegedly led to the son’s identification with the mother instead of the father, as was thought to occur for heterosexual men, and the first manifestation of this process was hypothesized to be cross-sex-typed behavior during childhood. Retrospective studies have generally found gay men to recall more distance from their fathers and more closeness to their

mothers during childhood (for reviews, see Freund & Blanchard, 1983; Friedman, 1988

); however, the effect sizes have been modest (e.g., Bell, Weinberg, & Hammersmith, 1981), and, more important, there is at least one alternative explanation for such findings. Because fathers are relatively intolerant of feminine behavior in their sons (e.g., Langlois & Downs, 1980), they may behave in a more distant or rejecting manner toward prehomosexual sons precisely because those sons are more likely to exhibit such behavior. By this account, childhood cross-sex-typed behavior is a cause rather than a consequence of “father distance.” Available data cannot definitively resolve which, if either, of these two possibilities explains the covariation among childhood sex-typed behavior, father distance, and sexual orientation for men. Biological theories of sexual orientation are more consistent with the possibility that childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation have common influences that precede (and, hence, are more likely to affect than be influenced by) family relationships.

To the extent that biological theories are supported, identification explanations become less plausible. Pillard (1990) provided evidence more directly relevant to this question. Heterosexual brothers of both homosexual and heterosexual index participants rated their closeness to their fathers during childhood. Brothers of homosexual probands rated their fathers as more distant than did the brothers of heterosexual participants, suggesting that the association between sexual orientation and father distance cannot be completely explained by fathers’ reactions to their homosexual sons’ cross-gender behavior. Pillard’s findings suggest that paternal traits may contribute to the emotional distance between gay men and their fathers, but prospective designs would be necessary to resolve the temporal sequence of boys’ cross-gender behavior and fathers’ emotional distance.

The second type of psychosocial explanation of the association between childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation stresses the role of parental socialization. Observational studies of parent–child interactions

have provided some evidence of sex-differentiated parental socialization during the toddler and preschool years (e.g., Fagot & Hagan, 1991 ; Fagot & Leinbach, 1989). Although parental socialization may account for some of the sex differences in childhood sex-typed behavior, these data are not directly related to differences between heterosexual and same-sex homosexual subjects, which are within-sex differences. Although there is some evidence that within-sex variation in the acquisition of sex-typed behavior is associated with parental socialization, this effect appears to diminish by the preschool years (see, e.g., Fagot & Leinbach, 1989

). Moreover, the range of behavioral variation is not great and is well within what might be described as the typical range for a child of a given sex. Green (1987)and colleagues (Roberts, Green, Williams, & Goodman, 1987) have examined socialization of sex-typed behavior among parents of feminine boys and reported that such parents were unusually tolerant of such behavior when it first emerged. These data are insufficient, however, to exclude the possibility that parents generally would be more tolerant than expected in the face of persistent cross-sex-typed behavior in their children.

Even if parental socialization accounted for the induction or maintenance of childhood cross-sex-typed behavior, the question would remain how such behavior is associated with, or converted to, a homosexual orientation. Green (1980) and Thorne (1986)suggested that the peer group might be an important context in which stimuli are eroticized. Green (1980), for example, speculated that feminine boys, because of their affiliative preference for girls, are more likely to assimilate the erotic fantasies about other boys common to the sexual rehearsal play of girls. A similar scenario could be constructed concerning masculine girls. Because little systematic empirical research has focused on the socialization of sexuality and eroticism, related hypotheses are difficult to evaluate.

Within-Orientation Differences in Childhood Sex-Typed Behavior

Although there were large mean sexual orientation differences in childhood sex-typed behavior for both sexes, there was also overlap. For the composite distributions we constructed, sexual orientation accounted for less than half the variance in childhood sex-typed behavior in both men and women (37% and 21%, respectively). Thus,

Ovid: Bailey: Dev Psychol, Volume 31(1).January 1995.43–55 5/14/03 7:25 AM

https://snap.it.northwestern.edu/p/p.cgi/ovidcom/gateway1:80/ovidweb.cgi Page 15 of 23

the bulk of the variance was within groups. Some of the within-group variance was undoubtedly caused by measurement error. Nevertheless, it seems likely that there are true within-group differences in childhood sextyped behavior for both heterosexual and homosexual participants. Such differences may be particularly pronounced among homosexual participants, who showed greater variability than heterosexual participants on relevant measures.

 

Etiological implications.

Any etiological theory of sexual orientation must account for, or at least allow, within-orientation differences in childhood sex-typed behavior. A neuroendocrine theory of sexual orientation might hypothesize that the brain

structures affecting childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation differentiate at similar, but not identical, times. Thus, any important fluctuations in androgens and other relevant hormones would be relatively likely to influence both sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation, but could also affect only one of them if sufficiently delimited.

Goy, Bercovitch, and McBrair (1988) provided some experimental support for this general idea in a study of prenatally androgenized female rhesus macaques. They varied the timing of prenatal exogenous exposure to testosterone propionate and found differential effects on selected sex-dimorphic behaviors. Compared with normal female rhesus macaques, early-exposed female rhesus macaques showed increased rates of maternal- and peer-mounting (male-typical behaviors) but did not differ in their rates of rough play; in contrast, late-exposed female rhesus macaques showed increased rates of rough play and peer-mounting but did not differ from normal female rhesus macaques in their rate of maternal-mounting. Goy et al. concluded that “the individual behavior traits that are components of the juvenile male role are independently regulated by the organizing action of androgen and have separable critical periods” (p. 552).

It is also conceivable that neuroendocrine theories might explain only homosexuality in individuals with childhood cross-sex-typed behavior and that different etiological hypotheses are needed to account for homosexual individuals who behaved more typically with respect to such behavior. For example, Bell et al. (1981) speculated that homosexuality not associated with cross-sex-typed behavior was more psychosocially determined. Because neither specific biological nor psychosocial influences on sexual orientation have been definitively demonstrated, there is no straightforward test of this hypothesis. Indirect tests have been attempted in both men and women with a behavior genetics framework (Bailey & Pillard, 1991; Bailey, Pillard, Neale, & Agyei, 1993). The likelihood that homosexual monozygotic twins were from concordant or discordant pairs did not depend on the extent of childhood cross-sex-typed behavior, suggesting that homosexuality associated with childhood crosssex-typed behavior is not especially genetic compared with other forms of homosexuality. Future etiological studies of sexual orientation should also assess childhood sex-typed behavior and should attempt to discern whether putative etiological factors affect sexual orientation alone or in combination with such behavior. For example, studies of women with CAH might assess both sexual orientation and childhood sex-typed behavior. If CAH is an appropriate model only for lesbians with childhood cross-sex-typed behavior, then lesbians with CAH should have a higher mean and restricted variance on retrospective measures of childhood sex-typed behavior compared with more representative samples of lesbians.

 

Developmental continuity of childhood sex-typed behavior.

A number of studies have assessed gender-related personality traits in

homosexual versus heterosexual adults using instruments such as the Bem Sex Role Inventory, the Personality Attributes Questionnaire, and the Masculinity–Femininity subscale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Although most of these studies have found homosexual adults to respond somewhat like opposite-sex heterosexuals on these scales (for a

review, see Pillard, 1991), they have generally ignored the question of whether adult personality differences are related to childhood differences in sex-typed behavior, that is, whether within orientations, the most sex-atypical adults were the most sex-atypical children. Two studies of homosexual adults found some support for continuity (Hooberman, 1979 ; Saghir & Robins, 1973), but further research on this question is desirable.

 

Other correlates of childhood sex-typed behavior.

Several characteristics have been found to correlate with childhood sex-typed behavior within either heterosexual or nonheterosexual (i.e., homosexual and bisexual) samples. Perhaps most relevant to this review, several studies have explored whether sex-typed behavior predicts gradations of sexual orientation within heterosexual or nonheterosexual groups. found that several items measuring sex-typed behavior correlated with degree of homosexual interest in a group of predominantly heterosexual men.

found similar results for nonheterosexual men. They reported that for each of 10 items regarding sextyped behavior, bisexual men were intermediate between heterosexual and gay men (sexual orientations were based on Kinsey ratings). A similar analysis by , however, failed to find such a relation in

non heterosexual men. , p. 208) found that bisexual women reported less “childhood gender nonconformity” than lesbians. found no relation between sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation within predominantly heterosexual women.

McConaghy and Silove (1991)

Phillips and

Over (1992)

Bailey (1989)

Bell et al. (1981

McConaghy and Silove (1991)

Results from several studies suggest that, for men, atypical patterns of sex-typed behavior during childhood were associated with negative adult outcomes. In the Fels Research Institute’s longitudinal study, , pp. 156–167) found that relatively masculine boys were less likely to show avoidance and inhibition regarding heterosexual erotic behavior in adulthood than were relatively unmasculine boys.

analyzed data from longitudinal study and found that men who had been relatively unmasculine boys were less well-adjusted socially and personally than were men who had been more typically masculine boys. Sexual orientation was not examined in either study; thus, it is unclear if these findings would remain if sexual orientation was controlled. Three studies of gay men found a similar link between childhood cross-sex-typed behavior and later problems, including lower self-esteem ( ), higher rates of depression and anxiety ( ), and suicidality ( ). None of the studies that included women found similar associations for them ( ; ; ), consistent with other work suggesting that cross-gender behavior in girls is less likely to have maladaptive social sequelae

( ). It is possible that the more negative outcomes for men reflect the greater social intolerance of crossgender behavior in boys and in men.

Kagan and Moss

(1962/1983

Aubé and Koestner (1992)

Sears, Maccoby, and Levin’s (1957)

Harry, 1983a

Weinrich, Atkinson, Grant, & The HNRC Group, 1992 Harry, 1983b

Aubé & Koestner, 1992 Harry, 1983b Kagan & Moss, 1983

Fagot, 1977

A final within-orientation correlate of sex-typed behavior concerns the sexual practices of gay men. Childhood cross-sex-typed behavior has been associated with a preference for receptive rather than penetrative anal sex (Saghir & Robins, 1973 ; Weinrich, Grant, et al., 1992); hence, gay men who exhibited cross-sex-typed behavior during childhood may be at increased risk for exposure to the human immunodeficiency virus.

 

Conclusión

Los individuos homosexuales recuerdan sustancialmente más comportamientos infantiles sexotipificados cruzados que los heterosexuales del mismo sexo. Los estudios prospectivos han dado apoyo a estos descubrimientos retrospectivos en relación con varones; quedan por hacer análogos estudios para mujeres. La futura investigación debería concentrarse en las causa de esta asociación, así como en las causas y la consecuencdia de la variación intraorientación en conductas sexotipificadas.

 

References

 

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the metaanalysis.

Allen, L. S., & Gorski, R. A. (1992). Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure of the human brain.

, , 7199–7202.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 89

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Aubé, J., & Koestner, R. (1992). Gender characteristics and adjustment: A longitudinal study. , , 485–

493.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63

Bailey, J. M. (1989). . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.

A test of the maternal stress hypothesis for human male homosexuality

*Bailey, J. M., & Benishay, D. S. (1993). Familial aggregation of female sexual orientation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 272–277.

*Bailey, J. M., & Gladue, B. A. (1993). [Sexual orientation and sex-dimorphic behaviors in men and women]. Unpublished raw data.

*Bailey, J. M., Miller, J. S., & Willerman, L. (1993). Maternally rated childhood gender nonconformity in homosexuals and heterosexuals.

, , 461–469.

Archives of Sexual Behavior 22

*Bailey, J. M., & Pillard, R. C. (1991). A genetic study of male sexual orientation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 1089–1096.

*Bailey, J. M., Pillard, R. C., Neale, M. C., & Agyei, Y. (1993). Heritable factors influence sexual orientation in women.

, , 217–223.

Archives of General Psychiatry 50

Bakwin, H. (1968). Deviant gender-role behavior in children: Relation to homosexuality. Pediatrics, 41, 620–629.

*Bell, A. P., Weinberg, M. S., & Hammersmith, S. K. (1981). . Bloomington: Indiana

University Press.

Sexual preference: Its development in men and women

Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. , ,

203–206.

Psychological Science 3

*Bieber, I., Dain, H. J., Dince, P. R., Drellich, M. G., Grand, H. G., Gundlach, R. H., Kremer, M. W., Rifkin, A. H., Wilbur, C. B., & Bieber, T. B.

(1962). Homosexuality: A psychoanalytic study of male homosexuals. New York: Basic Books.

*Blanchard, R., & Freund, K. (1983). Measuring masculine gender identity in females. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 205–214.

*Blanchard, R., McConkey, J. G., Roper, V., & Steiner, B. W. (1983). Measuring physical aggressiveness in heterosexual, homosexual, and

transsexual males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 12, 511–524.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1993). Psychopathology and early experience: A reappraisal of retrospective reports.

, , 82–98.

Psychological Bulletin 113

Brown, D. G. (1957). The development of sex-role inversion and homosexuality. Journal of Pediatrics, 50, 613–619.

Brown, D. G. (1958). Inversion and homosexuality. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 28, 424–429.

*Caretto, A. (1991). .

Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, Berkeley/Alameda.

Familial homosexuality among women and its relationship to gender role non-conformity in childhood and adult sex role

Carrier, J. M. (1986). Childhood cross-gender behavior and adult homosexuality [Letter to the editor]. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 15, 89–93.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the social sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

*Cole, M. M. (1983). . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, North Carolina State

University.

The developmental antecedents of sexual preference among males

Davenport, C. W. (1986). A follow-up study of 10 feminine boys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 15, 511–517.

Dittmann, R. W., Kappes, M. E., & Kappes, M. H. (1992). Sexual behavior in adolescent and adult females with

congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 17, 153–170.

Dittmann, R. W., Kappes, M. H., Kappes, M. E., Börger, D., Stegner, H., Willig, R. H., & Wallis, H. (1990). Congenital adrenal hyperplasia: I. Gender-related behavior and attitudes in female patients and sisters. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 15, 401–420.

Dörner, G. (1976). Hormones and brain differentiation. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Dynes, W. R. (Ed). (1987). Homosexuality: A research guide. New York: Garland.

Eaton, W. O., & Enns, L. R. (1986). Sex differences in human motor activity level. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 19–28.

Ehrhardt, A. A., & Baker, S. W. (1974). Fetal androgens, human central nervous system differentiation and behavior sex differences. In R. C. Friedman, R. M. Richart, & R. L. Vande Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behavior (pp. 33–51). New York: Wiley.

Ehrhardt, A. A., Evers, K., & Money, J. (1968). Influence of androgen and some aspects of sexually dimorphic behavior in women with the late treated adrenogenital syndrome. Johns Hopkins Medical Journal, 123, 115–122.

*Evans, R. B. (1969). Childhood parental relationships of homosexual men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 129–135.

Fagot, B. I. (1977). Consequences of moderate cross-gender behavior in preschool children. Child Development, 48, 902–907.

Fagot, B. I. (1985). Changes in thinking about early sex role development. Developmental Review, 5, 83–98.

Fagot, B. I., & Hagan, R. (1991). Observations of parent reactions to sex-stereotyped behaviors: Age and sex effects. , , 617–

628.

Child Development 62

Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1985). Gender identity: Some thoughts on an old concept. , ,

684–688.

Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 24

Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1989). The young child’s gender schema: Environmental input, internal organization. , , 663–

672.

Child Development 60

Freund, K., & Blanchard, R. (1983). Is the distant relationship of fathers and homosexual sons related to the sons’ erotic preference for male partners, or to the sons’ atypical gender identity, or to both? Journal of Homosexuality, 9, 7–25.

*Freund, K., & Blanchard, R. (1987). Feminine gender identity and physical aggressiveness in heterosexual and homosexual pedophiles.

, , 25–34.

Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy 13

*Freund, K., Langevin, R., Satterberg, J., & Steiner, B. (1977). Extension of the gender identity scale for males. , , 507– 519.

Archives of Sexual Behavior 6

*Freund, K., Nagler, E., Langevin, R., Zajac, A., & Steiner, B. (1974). Measuring feminine gender identity in homosexual males.

, , 249–260.

Archives of Sexual Behavior 3

Friedman, R. C. (1988). Male homosexuality: A contemporary psychoanalytic perspective. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

*Friedman, R. C., & Stern, L. O. (1980). Juvenile aggressivity and sissiness in homosexual and heterosexual males.

, , 427–440.

Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis 8

Gebhard, P. H. (1972). Incidence of overt homosexuality in the United States and Western Europe. In J. Livingood (Ed.),

(DHEW Publication No. HSM 72–9116, pp. 22–29). Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health.

NIMH Task Force on Homosexuality: Final report and background papers

Glass, G. V., McGaw, B., & Smith, M. L. (1981). Meta-analysis in social research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Goy, R. W., Bercovitch, F. B., & McBrair, M. C. (1988). Behavioral masculinization is independent of genital masculinization in prenatally androgenized female rhesus macaques. Hormones and Behavior, 22, 552–571.

Green, R. (1974). Sexual identity conflict in children and adults. New York: Basic Books.

Green, R. (1980). Patterns of sexual identity development in childhood: Relationship to subsequent sexual partner preference. In J. Marmor (Ed.), Homosexual behavior: A modern reappraisal (pp. 255–266). New York: Basic Books.

Green, R. (1985). Potholes on the research road to sexual identity development. Journal of Sex Research, 21, 96–101.

Green, R. (1987). The “sissy boy syndrome” and the development of homosexuality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

*Grellert, E. (1982). Childhood play behavior of homosexual and heterosexual men. Psychological Reports, 51, 607–610.

*Grellert, E. A., Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1982). Childhood play activities of male and female homosexuals and heterosexuals.

, , 451–478.

Archives of Sexual Behavior 11

*Guloien, E. H. (1983). . Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada.

Childhood gender identity and adult erotic orientation in males

*Gundlach, R. H., & Riess, B. F. (1968). Self and sexual identity in the female: A study of female homosexuals. In B. F. Riess (Ed.),

(pp. 205–231). New York: Grune & Stratton.

New directions in mental health

*Harry, J. (1982). Gay children grown up: Gender culture and gender deviance. New York: Praeger.

Harry, J. (1983a). Defeminization and adult psychological well-being among male homosexuals. , , 1–

19.

Archives of Sexual Behavior 12

Harry, J. (1983b). Parasuicide, gender, and gender deviance. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 350–361.

Harry, J. (1984). Sexual orientation as destiny. Journal of Homosexuality, 10, 111–124.

Harry, J. (1986). Sampling gay men. Journal of Sex Research, 22, 21–34.

*Harry, J. (1989). Parental physical abuse and sexual orientation in males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 18, 251–261.

Hedges, L. V. (1982). Estimation of effect size from a series of independent experiments. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 490–499.

*Hellman, R. E., Green, R., Gray, J. L., & Williams, K. (1981). Childhood sexual identity, childhood religiosity, and “homophobia” as influences in the development of transsexualism, homosexuality, and heterosexuality. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 910–915.

Herdt, G. (1990). Developmental discontinuities and sexual orientation across cultures. In D. P. McWhirter, S. A. Sanders, & J. M.

Reinisch (Eds.), Homosexuality/heterosexuality: Concepts of sexual orientation (pp. 208–236). New York: Oxford University Press.

*Hockenberry, S. L., & Billingham, R. E. (1987). Sexual orientation and boyhood gender conformity: Development of the Boyhood Gender Conformity Scale (BGCS). Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 475–492.

*Holemon, E. R., & Winokur, G. (1965). Effeminate homosexuality: A disease of childhood. , ,

48–56.

American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 35

*Hooberman, E. R. (1979). Psychological androgyny, feminine gender identity and self-esteem in homosexual and heterosexual males.

, , 306–315.

Journal of Sex Research 15

Hoult, T. F. (1983/1984). Human sexuality in biological perspective: Theoretical and methodological considerations. , ,

137–155.

Journal of Homosexuality 9

Huston, A. C. (1983). Sex-typing. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), (pp. 387–467). New York: Wiley.

Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development

Hyde, J. S. (1990). Meta-analysis and the psychology of gender differences. Signs, 16, 55–73.

Isay, R. A. (1989). Being homosexual: Gay men and their development. New York: Farrar Straus Giroux.

Kagan, J., & Moss, H. A. (1983). . New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. (Original work

published 1962).

Birth to maturity: A study in psychological development

*Kaye, H. E., Berl, S., Clare, J., Eleston, M. R., Gershwin, B. S., Gershwin, P., Kogan, L. S., Torda, C., & Wilbur, C. B. (1967). Homosexuality in

women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 17, 626–634.

*Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.

*Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953). . Philadelphia: W. B.

Saunders.

Sexual behavior in the human female

Kite, M. E., & Deaux, K. (1987). Gender belief systems: Homosexuality and the implicit inversion theory. , , 83–

96.

Psychology of Women Quarterly 11

Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive–developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.),

(pp. 82–173). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

The development of sex differences

Kohlberg, L., Ricks, D., & Snarey, J. (1984). Childhood development as a predictor of adaptation in adulthood. ,

, 91–172.

Genetic Psychology Monographs

110

Kosky, R. J. (1987). Gender-disordered children: Does inpatient treatment help? Medical Journal of Australia, 146, 565–569.

Langlois, J. H., & Downs, A. C. (1980). Mothers, fathers, and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviors in young children.

, , 1237–1247.

Child Development 51

Lebovitz, P. S. (1972). Feminine behavior in boys: Aspects of its outcome. American Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 1283–1289.

LeVay, S. (1991). A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men. Science, 253, 1034–1037.

McConaghy, N., & Silove, D. (1991). Opposite sex behaviours correlate with degree of homosexual feelings in the predominantly

heterosexual. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 77–83.

*McConkey, J. G. (1991).

. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

The triangular interrelation between sons’ erotic preference and physical aggression and the quality of father–son relationships

Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (1980). Sex hormone changes during puberty and sexual behavior. In J. Samson (Ed.), (pp. 113–122). Montreal, Quebec, Canada: Éditions Études Vivantes.

Childhood and sexuality

Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (1984). Psychoendocrine research on sexual orientation: Current status and future options. , ,

375–398.

Progress in Brain Research 61

Mischel, W. (1966). A social-learning view of sex differences in behavior. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 56–81). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Money, J. (1955). Hermaphroditism, gender and precocity in hyperadrenocorticism: Psychologic findings. , ,

253–264.

Bulletin of the Johns Hopkins Hospital 96

Money, J., & Russo, A. J. (1979). Homosexual outcome of discordant gender identity/role: Longitudinal follow-up.

, , 29–41.

Journal of Pediatric Psychology 4

Money, J., Schwartz, M., & Lewis, V. G. (1984). Adult erotosexual status and fetal hormonal masculinization and demasculinization: 46,XX congenital virilizing adrenal hyperplasia and 46,XY androgen-insensitivity syndrome compared. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 9, 405–414.

Mulaikal, R. M., Migeon, C. J., & Rock, J. A. (1987). Fertility rates in female patients with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21–hydroxylase deficiency. New England Journal of Medicine, 316, 178–182.

*Phillips, G., & Over, R. (1992). Adult sexual orientation in relation to memories of childhood gender conforming and gender nonconforming behaviors. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 543–558.

Pillard, R. C. (1990). The Kinsey scale: Is it familial? In D. P. McWhirter, S. A. Sanders, & J. M. Reinisch (Eds.),

(pp. 88–100). New York: Oxford University Press.

Homosexuality/heterosexuality: Concepts of sexual orientation

Pillard, R. C. (1991). Masculinity and femininity in homosexuality: “Inversion” revisited. In J. C. Gonsiorek & J. D. Weinrich (Eds.),

(pp. 32–43). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Homosexuality: Research implications for public policy

Pillemer, D. B., & White, S. H. (1989). Childhood events recalled by children and adults. In H. W. Reese (Ed.),

(Vol. 21, pp. 297–340). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Advances in child development and behavior

*Poole, K. (1972). The etiology of gender identity and the lesbian. Journal of Social Psychology, 87, 51–57.

Risman, B., & Schwartz, P. (1988). Sociological research on male and female homosexuality. Annual Review of Sociology, 14, 125–147.

Roberts, C. W., Green, R., Williams, K., & Goodman, M. (1987). Boyhood gender identity development: A statistical contrast of two family groups. Developmental Psychology, 23, 544–557.

Ross, M. W. (1980). Retrospective distortion in homosexual research. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 9, 523–531.

Ross, M. W. (1984). Beyond the biological model: New directions in bisexual and homosexual research. , ,

63–70.

Journal of Homosexuality 10

Ross, M. W., Rogers, L. J., & McCulloch, H. (1978). Stigma, sex, and society: A new look at gender differentiation and sexual variation.

, , 315–330. Journal of Homosexuality 3

*Saghir, M. T., & Robins, E. (1973). Male and female homosexuality: A comprehensive investigation. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

Sandberg, D. E., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Ehrhardt, A. A., & Yager, T. J. (1993). The prevalence of gender-atypical behavior in elementary school children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 306–314.

*Sanders, R. M., Bain, J., & Langevin, R. (1985). Feminine gender identity in homosexual men: How common is it? In R. Langevin (Ed.), (pp. 249–259). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Erotic preference, gender identity, and aggression in men: New research studies

Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Serbin, L. A. (1980). Sex-role socialization: A field in transition. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), (Vol. 3,

pp. 41–96). New York: Plenum.

Advances in clinical child psychology

Smith, M. L., Glass, G. V., & Miller, T. I. (1980). The benefits of psychotherapy. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

*Snortum, J. R., Gillespie, J. F., Marshall, J. E., McLaughlin, J. P., & Mosberg, L. (1969). Family dynamics and homosexuality.

, , 763–770.

Psychological Reports 24

*Stephan, W. G. (1973). Parental relationships and early social experiences of activist male homosexuals and male heterosexuals.

, , 506–513.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology 82

Stoller, R. J. (1964). The hermaphroditic identity of hermaphrodites. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 139, 453–457.

Storms, M. D. (1983). .

(Available from the Office of Social and Ethical Responsibility, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC)

Development of sexual orientation. Module of the Committee on Gay Concerns of the American Psychological Association

*Thompson, N. L., Schwartz, D. M., McCandless, B. R., & Edwards, D. A. (1973). Parent–child relationships and sexual identity in male and female homosexuals and heterosexuals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41, 120–127.

Thorne, B. (1986). Girls and boys together … but mostly apart: Gender arrangements in elementary schools. In W. W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and development (pp. 167–184). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

*Tkachuk, J., & Zucker, K. J. (1991, August).

. Poster presented at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, Barrie, Ontario, Canada.

The relation among sexual orientation, spatial ability, handedness, and recalled childhood gender

identity in women and men

Tweedy, R. W. (1984). . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, St. John’s University.

Homosexual signs on the Rorschach and gender identity, psychopathology, and overt sexual behavior

Unger, R. K. (1979). Toward a redefinition of sex and gender. American Psychologist, 34, 1085–1094.

Watt, N. F., Anthony, E. J., Wynne, L. C., & Rolf, J. E. (Eds.). (1984). . Cambridge,

England: Cambridge University Press.

Children at risk for schizophrenia: A longitudinal perspective

Weinberg, M. S., & Bell, A. P. (Eds.). (1972). Homosexuality: An annotated bibliography. New York: Harper & Row.

Weinrich, J. D., Atkinson, J. H., & Grant, I. The HNRC Group. (1992, July).

. Poster presented at the meeting of the

International Academy of Sex Research, Prague, Czechoslovakia.

Is gender dysphoria dysphoric? Elevated depression and anxiety in gender dysphoria in comparison with other types of homosexual and bisexual men in an HIV sample

Weinrich, J. D., Grant, I., Jacobson, D. L., Robinson, S. R., & McCutchan, J. A. The HNRC Group. (1992). Effects of recalled childhood gender nonconformity on adult genitoerotic role and AIDS exposure. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 21, 559–585.

*Whitam, F. L. (1977). Childhood indicators of male homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 6, 89–96.

*Whitam, F. L., & Mathy, R. M. (1986). . New York: Praeger.

Male homosexuality in four societies: Brazil, Guatemala, the Philippines, and the United States

*Whitam, F. L., & Mathy, R. M. (1991). Childhood cross-gender behavior of homosexual females in Brazil, Peru, the Philippines, and the United States. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20, 151–170.

Yarrow, M. R., Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R. V. (1970). Recollections of childhood: A study of the retrospective method.

, (5, Serial No. 138).

Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 35

Zucker, K. J. (1985). Cross-gender-identified children. In B. W. Steiner (Ed.), (pp. 75–174).

New York: Plenum Press.

Gender dysphoria: Development, research, management

Zucker, K. J. (1987). Commentary on Kohlberg, Ricks, and Snarey’s (1984) “Childhood Development as a Predictor of Adaptation in Adulthood.” Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 113, 127–130.

Zucker, K. J. (1990). Gender identity disorders in children: Clinical descriptions and natural history. In R. Blanchard & B. W. Steiner (Eds.), (pp. 1–23). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Clinical management of gender identity disorders in children and adults

Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Oliver, G., Hood, J. E., Blake, J., & Fleming, S. (1992, July).

. Poster presented at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, Prague,

Czechoslovakia.

Psychosexual assessment of women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia: Preliminary analyses

Zuger, B. (1984). Early effeminate behavior in boys: Outcome and significance for homosexuality. , , 90–

97. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 172

For three studies (Bell et al., 1981; Kinsey et al., 1948; Kinsey et al., 1953), effect sizes were computed by the Kinsey Institute using the studies’ raw data.

1